Plasticity In Human Behavior Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

Sample Plasticity In Human Behavior Research Paper. Browse other  research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. If you need a religion research paper written according to all the academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help. This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our research paper writing service for professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable rates.

The concept of plasticity originated in the medical sciences and refers to the capacity of organism to be modifiable. This variability and flexibility enables human beings to adapt to a great range of new and changing environmental conditions. Starting from this origin, the concept of plasticity has been applied to a broad scope of phenomena, the plasticity of nervous systems, changes in observable behavior patterns, and life trajectories.

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


Neuronal plasticity research, for example, addresses the biochemical and neuronal processes underlying the development and the variability of behavior over the lifespan; that is, it refers to the mechanisms of brain development that involve changes in structure and function (brain plasticity). A key concept in this field was the ‘critical period’ (Wiesel and Hubel 1963) which refers to a phase in the lifespan when neuronal structures are highly malleable based on the level and type of activity exposed to. For organisms at increasing maturity levels it was expected that neurostructural plasticity decreases with age and is absent in the mature cortex. However, subsequent research revealed substantial plasticity in the mature adult cortex, for example after lesions, strokes, and nerve damage or—even more important—after learning occurs (e.g., Buonomano and Merzenich 1998, Recanzone 2000). Studies showing activity-dependent plasticity on brain structures and transmitter systems in the aging brain provided convincing evidence that brain plasticity is not restricted to juvenile stages of development, but at least in the human species covers the entire lifespan (e.g., Cotman and Neeper 1996).

In contrast to neuronal plasticity research, the focus in lifespan psychological plasticity research lies on identifying the range and the conditions of behavioral modifiability as well as their age-related constraints across the entire life course (behavioral plasticity). In that context, the concept of plasticity generally refers to the ability of human beings to display rather different patterns of behavior as well as different life trajectories (Baltes and Willis 1982, Lerner 1984). So, plasticity can refer to variability of life course patterns (ontogenetic plasticity) or to the variability of a performance in a specific moment in time (concurrent plasticity). Moreover, the study of intraindividual behavioral variability is not restricted to cognitive functioning but extends to noncognitive domains such as motivation and personality. To understand changes in behavioral and ontogenetic plasticity across the lifespan, broader theoretical frameworks of human functioning have to be considered that take the dynamic and complex interplay between evolutionary/biology-based and culture-based influences on the nature of lifespan development into account (Baltes et al. 1998, Finch 1996).




1. Human Behavioral Plasticity As A Product Of Evolution And The Foundation Of Culture

Much of the behavioral and ontogenetic plasticity is based on the phylogeny of mammals, a biological strata characterized by open behavioral programs (Mayr 1974). With few exceptions, human behavior is not regulated by preprogrammed stimulus–response patterns, but entails the potential to adapt to a variety of ecologies and stimulus constellations. This plasticity opens behavioral patterns both in terms of concurrently available behavioral options and—even more important for ontogenetic change—in terms of acquiring new behavioral patterns. This holds too and is even accentuated when one views the evolution of the human mind from a perspective of evolved domain-specific behavioral and cognitive modules (Cosmides and Tooby 1994, Fodor 1983), which are open to ecological and cultural variations. In the course of human phylogeny such evolved mental modules have escaped domain-specific encapsulation (see Fodor’s original conception of encapsulated modules, Fodor 1983) and thus provided the building blocks of human intellectual capacities and behavioral adaptiveness. This variability in behavior and development is as much a product of evolution as the more narrowly constrained behavioral systems of other species (e.g., Wilson 1980).

Behavioral plasticity opens the potential for systematic influences of experiences, most notably influences of socialization and instruction. Both on the part of the socializing agent and recipient, flexible regulation of behavior in the interaction allows the partners to move beyond preformatted parenting and attachment programs of behavior, and reach out for the social—and ultimately cultural—transmission of experience between generations, and more generally between experts and novices. The ability to instruct and to profit from instruction probably is the critical difference in behavioral regulation which sets homo sapiens apart from other primates (Tomasello et al. 1993). Only humans seem to intentionally instruct their children (or nonexpert adults) to modify behavior in accordance with a standard or goal. They correct, model subcomponents, and thus selectively modify error-prone behavior of a partner to fit a certain standard. This specific instructional potential of human interactions is the vehicle of fidelity in information transmission (Tomasello et al. 1993), the prerequisite of cultural heritage. In modern societies this instructional potential has led to the institutionalization of instruction in educational institutions, such as schools and universities. Instructional interactions between humans and the training in educational systems have in turn greatly amplified the range of plasticity in human behavior. The combination of evolutionary potential and cultural transmission can thus generate an immense, albeit not infinite, universe of behavioral plasticity in many potential developmental pathways. Indeed, this great potential for variability, plasticity, and adaptivity in human ontogeny is a cornerstone of lifespan developmental theories (Baltes 1997, Lerner 1984).

2. Plasticity Of Performance Across The Lifespan

The range of plasticity in performance in diverse domains of functioning (e.g., athletic, intellectual, creative) is determined by the joint influences of either the genotype-related component of performance, that is, genetically-based interindividual differences in performance, or the phenotype-related component of performance, that is, intraindividual differences based on current performance factors (e.g., motivation, alertness), accumulated investment, and instruction. While genotype-related influences delimit the range of plasticity for any given individual, the phenotype-based influences determine whether this potential of plasticity is actually realized (Kliegl and Baltes 1987). Specifically, these four influences are the following. First, there are age-graded differences in the baseline opportunities for maximum performance. It is well known and empirically established that across the human lifespan, performance in various domains of functioning undergoes radical changes, typically reflecting an inverted U-shaped trajectory, with a maximum performance potential at a given age (Lehman 1953, Simonton 1988). Age of maximum performance is relatively early for most athletic disciplines, particularly the track-and-fields sports (Schulz and Curnow 1988), and moves further into adulthood for areas of peak performance which require extensive expertise, such as soloist musical performance (Ericsson et al. 1993), science, and poetry (Simonton 1988). Second, a great many contextual factors can influence an individual’s baseline reserve capacity (Kliegl and Baltes 1987). Thus, the component of reserve capacity which influences current performance can be optimized by improving motivation, alertness, clarity of task instruction, and other contextual factors. Albeit opening up many avenues for intervention to promote improved performance, these performance factors are problematic from the perspective of basic research (Salthouse 1985). The performance factors can vary with age and thus confound age differences, so that accurate assessments of latent performance potential at different ages is compromised. Baltes, Kliegl and their colleagues have proposed the methodology of Testing-the-Limits as a research strategy to assess cognitive reserve capacity in terms of latent performance potentials. This research paradigm optimizes performance factors for all individuals and assesses their ability to profit from a training designed to construct a specific cognitive skill, for which the different age groups do not hold differential experiences (Kliegl and Baltes 1987). This latter component of instruction-related plasticity of performance is the third influencing factor, addressed next. Third, performance can be optimized by providing appropriate instruction. The influence of instruction on performance is a longstanding research topic in diverse fields of study and approaches to psychological functioning. Vygotsky conceptualized the zone of proximate development as the range of potential performance an individual can attain with the help and instruction of an expert interactive partner (Vygotsky 1978). This conception about developmental reserve capacity in socially constructive interactions has sprawned much research on social facilitation in parent–child dyads (scaffolding, Wood et al. 1976; apprenticeship, Heckhausen 1987, Kaye 1982, Rogoff and Wertsch 1984), child–child interactions (Rogoff 1990), cognitive aging (development-enhancing interventions, Kliegl and Baltes 1987), and educational psychology (see zone of potential development, Brown and French 1979). Finally fourth, plasticity in performance is a function of the individual’s long-term investment in a given area of skill or expertise. Research on life-course trajectories of peak performance across a wide variety of domains has demonstrated repeatedly that world-class levels of performance require extensive and high-quality expert-guided training and practice over long periods of the lifespan, particularly in the early phases of acquisition (Ericsson and Charness 1994, Simonton 1988). This powerful effect of focuses and prolonged practice and training is itself the best witness of the impressive plasticity of human performance across the lifespan.

3. Cognitive Functioning In Old Age: A Case In Point

An extensively studied area of human behavioral plasticity is cognitive functioning in old age. Numerous studies have demonstrated declining performance in fluid intelligence performance with increasing age among older adults (Salthouse 1985). This pattern of age related decline has been challenged to be reflective of true latent performance potential, since many factors such as motivation or lack of recent practice could produce performance differences such as those found between older and younger adults in intellectual functioning. This critique prompted researchers to optimize performance conditions for older adults for instance by using ecologically valid tasks. Moreover, specifically designed training programs were administered to promote improved performance in older adults (Verhaeghen et al. 1992, Willis 1989). Examples of early training studies are among others the Penn State Adult Development and Enrichment Project (ADEPT) and the Berlin PROALT project (for review see, Baltes and Lindenberger 1988). These studies demonstrated substantial benefits of older adults’ fluid intelligence performance from cognitive training. For tasks that were similar to those addressed by the cognitive training, older adults improved their performance. However, the training effect showed no transfer to other fluid intelligence tasks. Additional studies comparing the benefit of guided instruction with self-regulated practice showed that self-instruction was as effective in improving cognitive performance in older adults as was guided instruction. This suggests that a large share of the old age plasticity in cognitive functioning is due to the activation of already available cognitive skills, which have been fallen into disuse over the adult lifespan.

The findings of preserved substantial learning potentials up to old age and the possibly arising optimistic view of unchanged cognitive plasticity over the entire lifespan had been put in perspective by Testing-the-Limits studies aiming to delimit maximum performance potentials (Kliegl and Baltes 1987). In this research, cognitive skills are taught to subjects using specific training programs to construct an expertise in a certain cognitive domain, for example in the domain of memory for nouns. In this way, an expertise is cognitively engineered, so that the researchers know the component abilities, and can thus specifically compare subjects at different ages in terms of specific subskills. Subjects are trained extensively until they have reached asymptotic performance levels. Once maximum performance levels are achieved, time constraints are introduced. The findings indicate that although older adults can reach high levels of performance (e.g., in recalling nouns), their maximum (asymptotic) performance level after extensive training is much lower compared to younger adults. Moreover, their functioning breaks down when they encounter severe time constraints. Thus, at limits of functioning, age differences are magnified and reveal the potentially underlying neurobiological constraints associated with the biology of the aging brain (Baltes and Kliegl 1992). Finally, Testing-the-Limits methodologies have been successfully employed to identify elderly at risk of dementia (Baltes et al 1992) and distinguish pathological from normal cognitive aging (Reischies and Lindenberger 1996).

4. Plasticity In Personality Across The Lifespan

Processes of self-regulation, personality, and subjective well-being show much less systematic age variation than cognitive functioning. Indeed, the stability of life satisfaction, self-esteem, perceived control, and overall happiness in itself is proof of the adaptive plasticity of these processes. The age-normative shift from a predominance of perceived gains to ever increasing risks for decline (Heckhausen et al. 1989) would lead one to expect concomitant declines in indicators and components of subjective well-being. Instead, emotional balance remains resilient across the life span (Staudinger et al. 1993).

This resilience of the emotional balance to lifespan changes is one of the key achievements of the human motivational system, which has evolved to manage the challenges associated with the relative dearth of biologically-based predetermination of behavior. The relatively weak biological regulation of behavior furnishes a regulatory need to achieve and maintain selectivity in behavioral investment, and to compensate for the ever-present failure and loss of a behavioral system that is underdetermined and thus highly error prone. Both selectivity and loss compensation, key concepts in lifespan developmental research (Baltes et al. 1998, Schulz and Heckhausen 1999, Carstensen et al. 1999), require adaptive motivational processes, which focus volitional investment into a chosen goal and buffer the potentially negative effects of agingrelated losses. Typical examples of such metamotivational processes (i.e., secondary control, Heckhausen and Schulz 1995) which facilitate goal striving, are upward social comparisons with superior or advanced others, who can serve as models for self-improvement (Heckhausen and Krueger 1993), and enhanced valuation of the chosen compared to competing goals. Examples of metamotivational processes, which compensate for the negative effects of losses or failures, are self-enhancing comparisons with inferior others, selfprotective attributions which take credit for gains but refuse to take blame for losses, and the adjustment of goals to fit one’s real potential (for the latter example see also Brandtstadter and Renner’s conception of accommodation, Brandtstadter and Renner 1992). In general, the age-graded structure of opportunities and constraints sets the stage for the individual to use the age window of maximal opportunities to strive for a given goal. The regulation of motivational investment requires substantial plasticity in that the individual needs to enter cycles of engagement and disengagement with various developmental goals (Heckhausen 1999). Motivational engagement and disengagement with specific life goals should be contingent and flexibly adapted to changes in opportunities and constraints across the lifespan.

Bibliography:

  1. Baltes M M, Kuhl K P, Sowarka D 1992 Testing for limits of cognitive reserve capacity: a promising strategy for early diagnostic of dementia? Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences 47: 165–7
  2. Baltes P B 1997 On the incomplete architecture of human antogeny: selection, organization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist 52: 366–80
  3. Baltes P B, Kliegl R 1992 Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psychology 28: 121–5
  4. Baltes P B, Lindenberger U 1988 On the range of cognitive plasticity in old age as a function of experience: 15 years of intervention research. Behavior Therapy 19: 283–300
  5. Baltes P B, Lindenberger U, Staudinger U M 1998 Life-span theory in developmental psychology. In: Lerner R M (ed.) Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 1: Theoretical Models of Human Development, 5th edn. J. Wiley, New York, pp. 1029–1143
  6. Baltes P B, Willis S L 1982 Plasticity and enhancement of intellectual functioning in old age: Penn State’s Adult Development and Enrichment Project (ADEPT). In: Craik F I M, Trehub S (eds.) Aging and Cogniti e Processes. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 353–89
  7. Brandtstadter J, Renner G 1992 Coping with discrepancies between aspirations and achievements in adult development: a dual-process model. In: Montada L, Filipp S-H, Lerner M R (eds.) Life Crises and Experiences of Loss in Adulthood. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 301–19
  8. Brown A L, French L A 1979 The zone of potential development: implication for intelligence testing in the year 2000. Intelligence 3: 255–77
  9. Buonomano D V, Merzenich M M 1998 Cortical plasticity: From synapses to maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience 21: 149–86
  10. Carstensen L L, Isaacowitz D M, Charles S T 1999 Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist 54: 165–81
  11. Cosmides L, Tooby J 1994 Origins of domain-specificity: the evolution of functional organization. In: Hirschfeld L A, Gelman S A (eds.) Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 85–116
  12. Cotman C W, Neeper S 1996 Activity-dependent plasticity and the aging brain. In: Schneider L, Rowe J W (eds.) Handbook of the Biology of Aging, 4th edn. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 283–99
  13. Ericsson K A, Krampe R T, Tesch-Romer C 1993 The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychology Review 100(3): 363–406
  14. Ericsson K A, Charness N 1994 Expert performance: its structure and aquisition. American Psychologist 49(8): 725–47
  15. Finch C E 1996 Biological bases for plasticity during aging of individual life histories. In: Magnusson D (ed.) The Life-Span Development of Individuals: Behavioral, Neurobiological and Psychosocial Perspective. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 488–511
  16. Fodor J A 1983 The Modularity of Mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
  17. Heckhausen J 1987 Balancing for weaknesses and challenging developmental potential: a longitudinal study of motherinfant dyads in apprenticeship interactions. Developmental Psychology 23: 762–70
  18. Heckhausen J 1999 Developmental Regulation in Adulthood: Age-Normative and Sociostructural Constraints as Adaptive Challenges. Cambridge University Press, New York
  19. Heckhausen J, Dixon R A, Baltes P B 1989 Gains and losses in development throughout adulthood as perceived by different adult age groups. Developmental Psychology 25: 109–21
  20. Heckhausen J, Krueger J 1993 Developmental expectations for the self and most other people: age grading in three functions of social comparison. Developmental Psychology 29: 539–48
  21. Heckhausen J, Schulz R 1995 A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review 102: 284–304
  22. Kaye K 1982 The Mental and Social Life of Babies. How Parents Create Persons. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
  23. Kliegl R, Baltes P B 1987 Theory-guided analysis of mechanisms of development and aging through testing-the-limits and research on expertise. In: Schooler C, Schaie K W (eds.) Cognitive Functioning and Social Structure over the Life Course. Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 95–119
  24. Lehman H C 1953 Age and Achievement. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ
  25. Lerner R M 1984 On the Nature of Human Plasticity. Cambridge University Press, New York
  26. Mayr E 1974 Behavior programs and evolutionary strategies. American Scientist 62: 650–9
  27. Recanzone G H 2000 Cerebral cortical plasticity: Perception and skill acquisition. In: Gazzaniga S (ed.) The New Cognitive Neurosciences, 2nd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 237–47
  28. Reischies F M, Lindenberger U 1996 Grenzen und Potentiale kognitiver Leistungsf-Ahigkeit im Alter [Limits and potentials of cognitive ability in old age]. In: Mayer K U, Baltes P B (eds.) Die Berliner Altersstudie. Akademie Verlag, Berlin, pp. 351–78
  29. Rogoff B 1990 Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context. Oxford University Press, New York
  30. Rogoff B, Wertsch J V (eds.) 1984 Children’s learning in the ‘‘zone of proximal development’’. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
  31. Salthause T A 1985 A Theory of Cognitive Aging. NorthHolland, Amsterdam
  32. Schulz R, Cunow C 1988 Peak performance and age among super athletes: track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis, and golf. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences 43: 113–20
  33. Schulz R, Heckhausen J 1999 Aging, culture and control: setting a new research agenda. Journal of Gerontology series B: Psychological Sciences 54: 139–45
  34. Simonton D K 1988 Age and outstanding achievement: What do we know after a century of research? Psychological Bulletin 104: 251–67
  35. Staudinger U M, Marsiske M, Baltes P B 1993 Resilience and levels of reserve capacity in later adulthood: Perspectives from life-span theory. Development and Psychopathology 5: 541–66
  36. Tomasello M, Kruger A C, Ratner H H 1993 Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 495–511
  37. Verhaeghen P, Marcoen A, Goossens L 1992 Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: A metaanalytic study. Psychology and Aging 7: 242–51
  38. Vygotsky L S 1978 Mind in Society. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
  39. Wiesel T N, Hubel D H 1963 Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. Journal of Neurophysiology 26: 1003–17
  40. Willis S L 1989 Improvement with cognitive training: Which dogs learn what tricks? In: Poon L W, Rubin D C, Wilson B A (eds.) Everyday Cognition in Adulthood and Late Life. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 545–69
  41. Wilson E O 1980 Sociobiology. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
  42. Wood D J, Bruner J S, Ross G 1976 The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 17: 89–100

 

Play And Development In Children Research Paper
Piaget’s Theory Of Human Development Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!