Collaborative Memory Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

Sample Collaborative Memory Research Paper. Browse other  research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. If you need a research paper written according to all the academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help. This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our research paper writing service for professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable rates.

Although ‘collaborative memory’ may be briefly defined as remembering activities involving multiple persons, a more precise delineation of the phenomenon is the major goal of this research paper. Accordingly, several key premises of the phenomenon of collaborative memory should be noted at the outset. These include the following: (a) collaborative memory refers to a form or characteristic of remembering activity rather than to a theoretical system or function of memory; (b) scientific analysis of collaborative memory performance is based in part on principles derived from, and complementary to, theories pertaining to individual-level memory phenomena; (c) as collaborative memory performance is often influenced by both cognitive and interactive processes, research methods and interpretations from neighboring disciplines may be consulted or incorporated; (d) contemporary collaborative memory research and theory is neutral (i.e., empirically open) to questions regarding the benefits and costs of multiperson remembering, and may exist independent of specific effects; and (e) that numerous everyday remembering activities occur in the context of other cooperating individuals provides a necessary but not sufficient rationale for investigating collaborative memory.

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


1. Phases and Forms of Collaborative Memory

Collaboration in remembering may be differentiated by (a) whether it occurs during the encoding or retrieval phase of individual-level memory, or (b) the form of memory (i.e., the system or task) in which it occurs (or to which it is addressed). Although no definitive frequency data are available, collaborative memory activities occur perhaps most commonly during a retrieval phase. Thus, n 1 individuals attempt to recall information to which they have been previously exposed and for which no single individual possesses a perfect representation. Collaborative memory during this phase is often directed at remembering a commonly experienced personal event (i.e., collaborative episodic memory) or an item of knowledge or information to which the individuals would likely have been exposed (i.e., collaborative semantic memory). In addition, collaborating in recalling an intention or to-be-performed action (i.e., collaborative prospective memory) may be an example. Family or friendship groups attempting collectively to reconstruct stories from their shared past provide numerous fascinating (and entertaining) examples of collaborative episodic remembering. Laboratory illustrations include collaborative remembering of verbal or nonverbal information. Partners attempting to cue one another in recalling historical knowledge, such as the major Kings of Sweden or the combatants and outcome of the Peloponnesian War, are collaboratively performing a semantic memory task.

In addition to retrieval, remembering activities in the context of other individuals may occasionally concentrate on the encoding phase, wherein individuals collectively work to record incoming information in ‘multisite’ temporary storage (i.e., collaborative working memory). To be sure, in an inescapable sense, the ‘memory’ that is being encoded ‘resides’ in one brain, in personal or individual-level storage. Thus, collaborative encoding involves incidental or cooperative activities that may enhance the probability that a to-be-remembered (TBR) item is recorded, and thereby accessible (i.e., retrievable) at a later date by one or more individuals. For example, partners may develop encoding mnemonics or divide the task in order to enhance the storage of rapidly incoming information.




Collaboration may occur if the to-be-remembered information is not immediately or comprehensively available to any single participating individual. That is, a deficit is present—i.e., the answer is known neither immediately nor definitively by one individual—there is no opportunity (or no need) for collaboration. In collaborative episodic or semantic memory, an assumption is that the individuals may offer both common and unique memories. In addition, in the process of collaboration some mutual cuing may occur, such that novel items are produced. In collaborative prospective memory, the deficit may be an anticipated one. An individual may be concerned about forgetting to perform a future action (e.g., birthday, appointment, message) and enlist a spouse to help them remember at an appropriate time. Although both retrieval and encoding phases may occur in the (presumably assistive) context of participating individuals, both remain fundamentally individual-level functions. In this sense, collaboration qualifies the process and products of remembering, but does not necessarily carry emergent properties.

2. Scope and Selected Conceptual Issue

For several decades, researchers in a surprising variety of fields have addressed aspects of everyday memory activity that appear to operate in the influential context of other individuals. Although some cross-field differences in terminology and assumptions exist, several disputed conceptual issues are common to them all. One still-unresolved issue concerns the extent to which collaboration is optimally or acceptably effective.

2.1 Scope of the Phenomenon

The phenomenon has been also called collective (e.g., Middleton and Edwards 1990), situated (e.g., Greeno 1998), group (e.g., Clark and Stephenson 1989), socially shared (e.g., Resnick et al. 1991), interactive (e.g., Baltes and Staudinger 1996), transactive (Wegner 1987), or collaborative (e.g., Dixon 1996) memory. The fields in which this phenomenon has historically been of interest include educational psychology, cognitive science, social-cognitive psychology, industrial-organizational psychology, child developmental psychology, and adult developmental psychology (recent reviews include Baltes and Staudinger 1996, Engestrom and Middleton 1998, Kirshner and Whitson 1997, Lave and Wenger 1991).

2.2 Continuing Theoretical and Research Issues

Common to these literatures is the basic fact that two or more individuals attend to the same set of learning or memory tasks and are working cooperatively (although not necessarily effectively) to achieve a recall-related goal. Notably, the members of the collaborating group can be variously passive listeners, conversational interactants, productive collaborators, seasoned tutors, counterproductive or even disruptive influences, or optimally effective partners. Therefore, according to the neutral definition of collaborative memory espoused in this research paper, no a priori assumptions are made about the effectiveness or logical priority of the memory-related interaction. It has long been clear that group processes can vary in their effectiveness and thus group products can vary in their accuracy and completeness (e.g., Steiner 1972).

The issue of the extent to which collaborative memory is effective has been evaluated from numerous perspectives for several decades. Indeed, much research has focused on this contentious issue (e.g., Dixon 1999, Hill 1982), with several key factors appearing to play a role in the observations and inferences. These factors include: (a) whether the participants are collaborative-interactive experts (e.g., friends or couples), (b) the type of outcome measure observed (i.e, a simple product such as total items recalled or a variety of recall-related products such as elaborations and inferences), (c) the extent to which the actual processes (e.g., strategic negotiations) and byproducts (e.g., affect and sharing) of the collaborative communication are investigated, and (d) the comparison or baseline by which the effectiveness of collaborative performance is evaluated. In general, little extra benefit is observed under conditions in which researchers reduce the dimensionality of the tasks, the familiarity of the interactants, the variety of the memory-related products measured, and the richness of the collaborative communication (e.g., Meudell et al. 1992). In contrast, evidence for notable collaborative benefit may be observed when researchers attend to collaborative expertise, multidimensional outcomes, measurement of actual collaborative processes, and comparisons accommodated to memoryimpaired or vulnerable groups (e.g., Dixon and Gould 1998).

Bibliography:

  1. Baltes P B, Staudinger U M (eds.) 1996 Interacti e Minds: Lifespan Perspecti es on the Social Foundation of Cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK
  2. Clark N K, Stephenson G M 1989 Group remembering. In: Paulus P B (ed.) Psychology of Group Influence, 2nd edn. L. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
  3. Dixon R A 1996 Collaborative memory and aging. In: Herrman D, McEvoy C, Hertzog C, Hertel P, Johnson M K (eds.) Basic and Applied Memory Research. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ
  4. Dixon R A 1999 Exploring cognition in interactive situations: The aging of N 1 minds. In: Hess T M, Blanchard-Fields F (eds.) Social Cognition and Aging. Academic Press, San Diego, CA
  5. Dixon R A, Gould O N 1998 Younger and older adults collaborating on retelling everyday stories. Applied De elopmental Science 2: 160–71
  6. Engestrom Y 1992 Interactive expertise: Studies in distributed intelligence. Research Bulletin 83. Department of Education, University of Helsinki, Finland
  7. Engestrom Y, Middleton D (eds.) 1998 Cognition and Communication at Work. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK
  8. Greeno J G 1998 The situativity of knowing, learning, and research. American Psychologist 53: 5–26
  9. Hill G W 1982 Group versus individual performance: Are N 1 heads better than 1? Psychological Bulletin 91: 517–39
  10. Kirshner D, Whitson J A (eds.) 1997 Situated Cognition. L. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ
  11. Lave J, Wenger E 1991 Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK
  12. Meudell P R, Hitch G J, Kirby P 1992 Are two heads better than one? Experimental investigations of the social facilitation of memory. Applied Cogniti e Psychology 6: 525–43
  13. Middleton D, Edwards D (eds.) 1990 Collecti e Remembering. Sage, Newbury Park, CA
  14. Resnick L B, Levine J M, Teasley (eds.) 1991 Perspecti es on Socially Shared Cognition, 1st edn. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC
  15. Steiner I D 1972 Group Process and Producti ity. Academic Press, New York
  16. Wegner D M 1987 Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In: Mullin B, Goethals G R (eds.) Theories of Group Beha ior. Springer-Verlag, New York
Memory Development in Children Research Paper
Consolidation of Memory Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!