Prosocial Behavior And Empathy Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

Sample Prosocial Behavior And Empathy Research Paper. Browse other  research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. If you need a religion research paper written according to all the academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help. This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our research paper writing service for professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable rates.

Research on helping others in distress took off with the brutal murder of a young woman in Queens, New York in the mid-1960s, which was witnessed by 30 people from their apartment windows. The initial research on why witnesses do not help (Latane and Darley 1970) focused on the context and found that the presence of other bystanders can interfere with a person helping by activating certain assumptions—‘diffusion of responsibility’ (I’m sure someone has called the police) or ‘pluralistic ignorance’ (no one is doing anything, so it can’t be an emergency). There was also research on when people do help, which showed among other things that the tendency of bystanders to help increases with age.

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


Recent research has focused more on prosocial motivation and emotion. It highlights empathic emotion, sympathy, and guilt. This research paper is concerned with developmental processes in empathic emotions and their contribution to helping. Also covered is the role of cognition in arousing, developing, and shaping empathic emotion and enlarging the individual’s perspective on prosocial behavior. Finally, the paper deals with the contribution of socialization especially by parents and peers, and gender differences.

1. Definition Of Empathy

Empathy has been defined in two ways: (a) the cognitive awareness of another person’s internal states (thoughts, feelings, perceptions, intentions); (b) the vicarious affective response to another person. This chapter deals with affective empathy, because of its motive properties (see Ickes 1997 for cognitive empathy).




Affective empathy was initially defined as a match between the observer’s and the model’s feeling. It is now defined as it is here, more in terms of the processes that make an observer’s feelings congruent with the model’s situation not his own (Hoffman 1978, 2000). There may be a match, but not necessarily as when one feels empathic anger on seeing someone attacked even when the victim feels sad or disappointed rather than angry. Most of the research on affective empathy as a prosocial motive focuses on empathic distress because prosocial moral action usually involves helping someone in discomfort, pain, danger, poverty, and the like.

2. Modes Of Empathic Arousal

Five empathy-arousal modes have been identified (Hoffman 1978). Three are primitive, automatic, and preverbal. (a) Mimicry, which has two steps: one spontaneously imitates another’s facial, vocal, or postural expressions of feeling, producing changes in one’s own facial or postural musculature; these changes trigger afferent feedback to the brain, which results in feelings that resemble the other’s feelings. (b) Classical conditioning, in which empathic distress becomes a conditioned response to distress in others through observing others in distress at the same time that one is in actual distress. (c) Direct association of cues from others or their situation with one’s own painful past experience. The empathy aroused by these three modes is passive, involuntary, based on the pull of surface cues, and requires little cognitive processing. Nevertheless, these modes are important because (a) they show that a person’s distress can indeed result from someone else’s painful experience; (b) they make empathy arousal possible in the early preverbal years of childhood; and (c) they give empathy an involuntary dimension throughout life.

Two empathy-arousing modes are higher order cognitive. (a) Verbally mediated association: language communicates the other’s distress and connects the other’s situation to one’s own painful past experience. (b) Perspective-taking: one feels something of the victim’s distress by imagining how one would feel in the victim’s place and/or trying to imagine directly how the victim feels by using information one has about him or her. These higher order cognitive modes may be drawn out over time and subject to voluntary control, but they may also be triggered immediately on witnessing another’s distress. What they contribute to empathy is scope; they also allow one to empathize with others who are not present.

Multiple modes are important because they enable observers to respond empathically to whatever distress cues are available (Hoffman 2000). Cues from the victim’s face, voice, or posture can be picked up through mimicry, situational cues, through conditioning or association. If a victim expresses distress verbally or in writing, or someone else describes the victim’s situation, one can empathize through verbally mediated association or perspective-taking. Empathic distress is thus a multidetermined hence reliable human response, which fits the argument that it survived natural selection and has a hereditary component (Hoffman 1981, Zahn-Waxler et al. 1992).

3. Developmental Stages

Mature empathy has a metacognitive dimension: one knows one’s feeling of distress results from another’s plight and how the other presumably feels. One thus has a sense of oneself and others as separate beings with independent inner states (that are only partly reflected in outward behavior), separate identities, and separate life conditions. Before 6 or 7 years children can empathize but without this metacognitive dimension. This suggests that empathic affect develops along with cognitive self other development. Five stages of empathy development have been hypothesized (Hoffman 1978, 2000).

(a) Global empathic distress: newborns show a vigorous, agitated distress response to another infant’s cry. (b) Egocentric empathic distress: one responds to another’s distress as though oneself were in distress. This occurs when children can be empathically aroused by the three preverbal modes but still lack a clear self other distinction. (c) Quasiegocentric empathic distress: one knows the other is in actual distress but confuses the other’s inner states with one’s own and tries to help by doing for the other what would comfort oneself. (d) Veridical empathic distress: one’s feeling is closer to the other’s because one realizes the other has inner states independent of one’s own. (e) Empathy for another’s experience beyond the immediate situation (e.g., chronic illness, economic hardship, deprivation): this high empathy level is possible when children realize that others have an identity and a generally sad or happy life condition; and empathy with an entire group (homeless; Oklahoma City bombing victims).

4. Causal Attribution’s Shaping Of Empathic Affect

Humans spontaneously attribute causality to events (Weiner 1985) and they presumably do this when witnessing someone in distress. If they blame the victim, empathic distress is of course reduced. Other-wise, depending on the attribution, empathic affect may be shaped into four empathy-based moral affects (Hoffman 2000). (a) Sympathetic distress, when the cause is beyond the victim’s control (illness, accident, loss). (b) Empathic anger, when someone else is the cause. (c) Empathic feeling of injustice, when a discrepancy exists between the victim’s character and the victim’s fate (a good person fares badly). (d) Empathy-based guilt over inaction, when one does not help or one’s efforts to help fail and the victim’s distress continues; here one’s empathic distress combines with blaming oneself not for causing the victim’s distress but allowing it to continue.

5. Empathy As A Prosocial Motive

Empathy correlates positively with helping. There is also experimental evidence that arousal of empathic distress leads to helping; and that observers help more quickly the greater the victims’ pain and the higher the observers’ empathic distress. Furthermore, observers’ empathic distress becomes less intense and they feel better if they help, but not if they don’t help or if despite their best efforts and through no fault of their own, the victim’s distress is not alleviated, which implies that the main objective of empathy-based helping is to alleviate the victim’s distress. There is also evidence that empathy reduces both aggressive and manipulative (Machiavellian) behavior. It thus seems clear that empathic distress is a prosocial motive. See reviews by Hoffman (1978, 2000) and Eisenberg and Miller (1987).

Empathy has limitations, however (Hoffman 1984, 2000). (a) Egoistic motives may intervene and one may not help, even at the price of feeling guilty afterwards. (b) Empathic distress increases with the level of victims’ distress, but empathic distress can become aversive enough to shift one’s attention from the victim’s to one’s own personal distress (empathic overarousal). (c) People empathize with almost anyone in distress (the victims in research are usually strangers), but they empathize more with kin, in-group members, friends, and people who share their personal concerns (familiarity bias)—which may not be a problem except when intergroup rivalry contributes to intense empathic anger (hatred, ethnic cleansing) toward outgroup members. (d) People empathize more with victims who are physically present (here-and-now bias), probably because most empathy-arousing processes require immediate situational and personal cues.

6. Socialization By Parents And Peers

The role of parent as socializer has three facets: model, disciplinarian, nurturer. Each contributes to children’s empathy and prosocial behavior, although discipline has been the most studied (Hoffman 2000). The discipline method most likely to contribute to empathy, helping, and guilt over harming others is a type of reasoning called induction, often used by educated middle-class American parents when children harm or are about to harm someone. Inductions direct the child’s attention to the victim’s distress and may thus engage the empathy-arousing modes noted above. They also highlight the child’s role in causing the victim’s distress, which may lead to empathy-based transgression guilt. Power-assertive disciplines (physical force, threats, commands) are associated with low empathy, low helping, and low guilt, although some power assertion may be needed at times to get children to attend and process an induction’s message.

Parents’ behavior outside discipline encounters may provide prosocial models that reinforce children’s empathic proclivity and legitimize helping (Eisenberg and Fabes 1998, Hoffman 2000). Examples are expressing compassion toward people in difficult straights such as the homeless; linking a television protagonist’s feelings or situations with the child’s own experience; pointing out similarities among all humans such as sadness due to separation and loss. Parent models may thus reinforce children’s empathic dispositions especially when they are bystanders and someone needs help. Parent models may also make children more receptive to inductions, and, along with inductions, make children more receptive to claims of their peers (Hoffman 2000).

Piaget suggested in the 1930s that interaction with peers (not parents, who are too powerful) fosters prosocial moral development in children (Piaget 1932). Cognitive-developmental psychologists have recently elaborated this view (Turiel 1998), briefly as follows: children ‘co-construct’ and discover moral norms in the give-and-take of peer interaction (disputes, arguments, negotiations). These peer conflicts put pressure on children to take different points of view and to consider and coordinate the needs and rights of self and others (for example, regarding claims to ownership and possession of objects). This is a compelling hypothesis that merits research. Hoffman (2000) suggests that the peer interaction processes involved can serve to add the finishing touches to the prosocial outcomes of having inductive, nurturant, prosocial models as parents.

7. The Role Of Cognition

Though we deal with empathic affect, cognition’s importance is evident in two empathy arousal processes (verbally mediated association and perspective taking): in the key role of self other differentiation in empathy development; in the production of empathic anger and empathy-based guilt through causal attribution; and in children processing the information in inductions. Beyond that, cognitive development enables humans to form images, represent people and events, and imagine themselves in another’s place; and because represented people and events can evoke affect, victims need not be present for empathy to be aroused in observers. Empathy can be aroused when observers imagine or read about victims, discuss or argue about economic or political issues that involve victims, or make moral judgments about hypothetical moral dilemmas in which there are victims. See Hoffman (1987, 2000) for discussion of the importance of cognition, including the cognitive dimension of moral principles, in empathy’s development and prosocial functioning.

8. Gender Differences

Girls are socialized more than boys to be kind and feel responsible for others’ well-being. Girls are thus expected, increasingly with age, to be more empathic and prosocial and less likely to harm others. The research supports these expectations but in varying degrees (Eisenberg and Fabes 1998). Girls show more kindness and consideration for others and, to a lesser extent, are more likely to help and share. They are less likely to harm others and more apt to feel guilty when they do. They are also more empathic than boys, but this is clear only with self-report, especially questionnaire measures, in which it is obvious what is being assessed and responses are under the subject’s control. It is not clear with unobtrusive naturalistic observations or physiological indices of empathy. The empathy findings may thus reflect subjects’ conceptions of what boys and girls are supposed to be like. Whether they reflect gender differences in actual behavior is a matter for future research to decide.

Bibliography:

  1. Eisenberg N, Fabes R A 1998 Prosocial development. In: Damon W (ed.) Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, 5th edn. Wiley, New York, vol. 3
  2. Eisenberg N, Miller P A 1987 The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin 101: 91–119
  3. Hoffman M L 1978 Empathy, its development and prosocial implications. In: Keasey C B (ed.) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, NV. Vol. 25, pp. 169–218
  4. Hoffman M L 1981 Is altruism part of human nature? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40: 121–37
  5. Hoffman M L 1984 Empathy, its limitations, and its role in a comprehensive moral theory. In: Kurtines W, Gewirtz J (eds.) Morality, Moral Behavior, and Moral Development. John Wiley, New York, pp. 283–302
  6. Hoffman M L 1987 The contribution of empathy to justice and moral judgment. In: Eisenberg N, Strayer J (eds.) Empathy and its Development. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 47–80
  7. Hoffman M L 2000 Empathy and its Development: Implications for Caring and Justice. Cambridge University Press, New York
  8. Ickes W 1997 Empathic Accuracy. Guilford Press, New York
  9. Latane B, Darley J M 1970 The Unresponsive Bystander: Why Doesn’t He Help? Appleton-Century Crofts, New York
  10. Lennon R, Eisenberg N 1987 Gender and age differences in empathy and sympathy. In: Eisenberg N, Strayer J (eds.) Empathy and its Development. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 195–217
  11. Piaget J 1932 The Moral Judgment of the Child. Harcourt, New York
  12. Turiel E 1998 The development of morality. In: Damon W (ed.) Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. Wiley, New York, Vol. 3
  13. Weiner B 1985 ‘Spontaneous’ causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin 97: 74–84
  14. Zahn-Waxler C, Emde R N, Robinson J L 1992 The development of empathy in twins. Developmental Psychology 28: 1038–47
Psychology Of Prospective Memory Research Paper
Propositional Representations In Psychology Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!