Cultural Psychology Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

Sample Cultural Psychology Research Paper. Browse other research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. iResearchNet offers academic assignment help for students all over the world: writing from scratch, editing, proofreading, problem solving, from essays to dissertations, from humanities to STEM. We offer full confidentiality, safe payment, originality, and money-back guarantee. Secure your academic success with our risk-free services.

1. Cultural Psychology: What Is It?

A major aim of the discipline called cultural psychology is to document variations in modes of (and ideals for) ‘normal’ psychological functioning across ethnic communities. One assumption of the discipline—the premise of non-uniformity—is that there is a substantial portion of the psychological nature of human beings that is neither homogeneous nor fixed across time and space. Cultural psychology seeks to document the protean cultural aspects of human psychological nature. The discipline can be defined as the study of the distinctive mentalities of particular peoples (Balinese Hindus, Satmar Hasidim, Chinese Mandarins) (see Boesch 1991, Geertz 1973, Greenfield and Cocking 1994, Markus et al. 1996, Miller 1997, Shweder 1991, Shweder and LeVine 1984, Shweder et al. 1998).

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


Cultural psychology can thus be distinguished from the study of general psychology, which is the study of mental structures and processes that are so widely distributed as to characterize the normal psychological functioning of all human beings (or perhaps even non-human primates as well). Research in cultural psychology has, for example, systematically corroborated the special status accorded to the defense of female ‘honor’ in the mentality of many Southern American white males (Nisbett and Cohen 1995). Research in cultural psychology has authenticated the claim that there is a positive sense of empowerment and a feeling of virtue associated with ‘modesty’ and the attitude of ‘respectful restraint’ in the psychology of women in some regions of the contemporary non-Western world (Menon and Shweder 1998). Such feelings of power and goodness associated with ‘modesty’ contrast with ideas about (and ideals for) psychological functioning constructed in the contemporary Anglo-American cultural region. Research findings in cultural psychology thus raise provocative questions about the integrity and value of alternative forms of subjectivity across cultural groups.

A second assumption of cultural psychology is that many mental states (and some mental processes) are best understood as by-products of the never-ending attempt of particular groups of people to understand themselves and to make manifest their self-under-standings through social practices (Bruner 1990, Geertz 1973, Wierzbicka 1993, 1999). That might be called the premise of self-reflexive social construction. Whether one studies Inuit Eskimos or Anglo-American middle-class urban liberals, the aim in cultural psychology is to spell out the implicit meanings (the goals, values, and pictures of the world) that give shape to psychological processes (Briggs 1970, White and Kirkpatrick 1985). The aim is to examine the patchy or uneven distribution of those meanings on a global scale and to investigate the manner of their social acquisition, for example by means of participation in the symbolic practices, including linguistic practices, of this or that tradition-sensitive cultural group.




2. The Mental States Of ‘Others’

One of the several aims of cultural psychology is to develop a language for the comparative study of mental states that makes it possible to understand and appreciate the mental life of ‘others.’ ‘Others’ refers to members of some different cultural community who by virtue of life-long membership in that group ascribe meaning to their lives in the light of wants, feelings, values, and beliefs that are not necessarily the same as one’s own.

Cultural psychologists are interested, for example, in cultural variations in the degree to which feelings are constructed as ‘emotions.’ Anna Wierzbicka (1999) has suggested that while all normal human beings have ‘feelings’ (e.g., pleasure and pain, arousal and serenity) many of the ‘emotions’ lexicalized in the English language are not universally available in the mental life of people around the world. There has been much research in cultural psychology into the character of the particular emotions (Ifaluk ‘fago,’ American ‘happiness’) that are salient or important in different types of social worlds (Lutz 1988, Kitayama and Markus 1994, Shweder and Haidt 2000).

Cultural psychologists are interested as well in population-based variations in social cognition, moral judgment, and the sources of personal fulfillment or life-satisfaction in different social groups. For example, they have studied the origin, significance, and place of ‘filial piety’ and the ‘social motivation to achieve’ in some East Asian populations (Yang 1997). They have investigated the self-empowering aspects of ascetic denial and other forms of sacrifice among high- caste women in South Asia (Menon 2000). They are concerned to document the divergent meanings and distinctive somatic and affective vicissitudes of such experiences as ‘loss’ (or ‘success,’ or of ‘not getting the things you want,’ etc.) for members of different cultural communities. It is in the pursuit of such research questions that they have discovered replicable cultural differences in reports about the quality of the experience of ‘loss’ (and ‘gain’). In comparison to majority populations in northern Europe or the USA, majority populations in Samoa and China are more likely to react to apparent ‘loss’ with feelings such as headaches, backaches, and other types of physical pain than with feelings such as sadness or dysphoria (Levy 1973, Kleinman 1986). Cultural psychologists also seek to document differences in modes of thought (e.g., analytic vs. holistic) (Nisbett et al. in press), in self-organization (e.g., interdependent vs. independent) (Markus and Kitayama 1991) and in moral judgment (e.g., reliance on an ethics of autonomy vs. an ethics of community vs. an ethics of divinity) across different types of groups (Shweder et al. 1997).

In most of its research the field of cultural psychology has been pluralistic in its conception of ‘normal’ psychological functioning and interdisciplinary in its conception of how to go about studying the origin, meaning, and social role of particular mental states on a worldwide scale. The field draws together anthropologists, psychologists, linguists, biologists, and philosophers in its study of the diverse, yet potentially effective, modes of psychological functioning that have been produced, and socially endorsed, in different cultural traditions, and have made those cultural traditions possible. Indeed, cultural psychology is sometimes described as the study of the way culture and psyche ‘make each other up.’

3. The Tenet Of Psychic Pluralism

Taken together, the premise of non-uniformity and premise of self-reflexive social construction sum up to a principle that is at the heart of the discipline of cultural psychology. This is the tenet of psychic pluralism, which states that the study of ‘normal psychology’ is the study of multiple psychologies, and not just the study of a single or uniform fundamental psychology for all peoples of the world. Cultural psychology is thus the study of the way the human ‘mind’ (understood to consist of an inherently complex, heterogeneous collection of abstract and/or latent schemata) can be transformed, and made functional, in a number of different ways, which are not equally distributed across time or space. Hence the slogan, ‘one mind, many mentalities: universalism without the uniformity’ (Shweder et al. 1998) which is meant to give expression to goals of a discipline aimed at developing a credible theory of psychological pluralism.

Cultural psychology can also be understood as a project designed to critically assess the limitations and incompleteness of all uniformitarian versions of the idea of psychic unity. Alternatively put, cultural psychology is the study of ethnic and cultural sources of diversity in emotional and somatic functioning, self-organization, moral evaluation, social cognition, and human development. It is the study of population differences in the things people know, think, want, feel, and value, and hence are customarily motivated to do, by virtue of life-long membership in some recognizable group that has a history and a conception of its own destiny. ‘To be a member of a group,’ the eighteenth-century German romantic philosopher Johann Herder argued, ‘is to think and act in a certain way in the light of particular goals, values and pictures of the world; and to think and act so is to belong to a group’ (Berlin 1976). Although the field of cultural psychology has many ancestral spirits (Giambattista Vico, Wilhelm Wundt, Wilhelm Dilthey, Edward Sapir, Ruth Benedict) and some very prominent contemporary advocates (Jerome Bruner, Michael Cole, Clifford Geertz, Jacqueline Goodnow, Patricia Greenfield, Shinobu Kitayama, Hazel Markus, Richard Nisbett, Hazel Markus, Anna Wierzbicka), Johann Herder is justly claimed as one of the original cultural psychologists, although he was probably not the first. For an account of the historical development of the field, see Jahoda (1991) and Cole (1996).

4. Culture And The ‘Custom Complex’

One of the contributions of cultural psychology is to revive a conception of ‘culture’ that is both symbolic and behavioral. In the history of twentieth-century anthropological thought, the idea of culture was variously defined, either behaviorally (as patterns of behavior that are learned and passed on from generation to generation) or ‘symbolically’ (as the categories, beliefs, and doctrines that organize, rationalize, and justify a way of life). In research on the cultural psychology of a particular cultural community, the notion of ‘culture’ usually refers to community-specific ideas about what is true, good, beautiful, and efficient that are made manifest in behavior. To qualify as ‘cultural’ those ideas about truth, goodness, beauty, and efficiency must be socially inherited and customary; and they must actually be constitutive of different ways of life, and play a part in the self-understanding (including the self-criticism) of members of the community.

Alternatively stated, the concept of ‘culture’ as used in cultural psychology refers to what Isaiah Berlin called ‘goals, values and pictures of the world’ that are made manifest in the speech, laws, and routine practices of some self-monitoring group. These are sometimes also called ‘cultural models’ (D’Andrade 1995, Shore 1996). Cultural psychologists thus engage in the interpretive, symbolic, or cognitive analysis of behavior. They assume that actions speak louder than words and that ‘practices’ are a central unit for cultural analysis. Thus, what John Whiting and Irvin Child (1953) once referred to as the ‘custom complex’ is a natural unit of analysis or starting point for a study in cultural psychology.

Whiting and Child introduced the idea of a ‘custom complex’ in 1953 but the basic idea was not really taken up and carried forward until the rebirth of cultural psychology in the 1980s and 1990s. According to Whiting and Child, a custom complex ‘consists of a customary practice [for example, a family meal, arranged marriage, animal sacrifice, a gender identity ceremony involving circumcision] and of beliefs, values, sanctions, rules, motives and satisfactions associated with it.’ There are many labels that have been placed on this type of unit of analysis (a ‘custom complex,’ a ‘life space,’ a ‘habitus’). Whatever the label, this is the type of unit of analysis that makes it possible to conceptualize cultural psychology as the study of the way culture (ideas about what is true, good, beautiful, and efficient made manifest in practice) and psyche (what people know, think, feel, want, value, and hence choose to do) afford each other realization, and thus ‘make each other up.’

5. An Example Of A ‘Custom Complex’: Genital Surgeries In Africa

A rather dramatic but highly illuminating example of a ‘custom complex’ is the ‘circumcision ceremony’ or ‘genital surgery’ that is customary for both boys and girls in many East and West African ethnic groups. In the countries of Sierra Leone, Mali, the Gambia, Ethiopia, Somalia, the Sudan, and Egypt genital surgeries are culturally endorsed and popular for both males and females, and receive high approval ratings from the vast majority of men and women in the general population. Human rights advocacy groups in Europe and in the USA have criticized the practice. Nevertheless, the most comprehensive and rigorous review of the medical and demographic literature on African genital surgeries suggests that widely publicized claims about the severe and negative consequences of the custom for health, sexuality, and childbirth are quite inaccurate, and deserve to be discounted (Obermeyer 1999). In a recent manuscript on the cultural psychology of male and female circumcision among the Kono people of Sierra Leone, Fuambai Ahmadu, who is both a ‘native’ and an anthropologist, has written as follows (Ahmadu 2000, p. 301).

It is difficult for me—considering the number of these ceremonies I have observed, including my own—to accept that what appear to be expressions of joy and ecstatic celebrations of womanhood in actuality disguise hidden experiences of coercion and subjugation. Indeed, I offer that most Kono women who uphold these rituals do so because they want to—they relish the supernatural powers of their ritual leaders over against men in society, and they embrace the legitimacy of female authority and, particularly, the authority of their mothers and grandmothers.

Among the various goals, values, and pictures of the world (the cultural psychology) that make this practice meaningful and satisfying for those men and women for whom it is a ‘custom complex’ are the following:

(a) A culturally shared belief that the body (especially the genitals) of both males and females are sexually ambiguous until modified through surgical intervention. According to this picture of the world the foreskin of a boy is viewed as a feminine element and masculinity is enhanced by its removal. Similarly the clitoris is viewed as an unwelcome vestige of the male organ, and Kono females (for example), as described by Ahmadu, seek to feminize and hence empower themselves by getting rid of what they perceive as a dispensable trace of unwanted male anatomy.

(b) A culturally shared aesthetic standard in terms of which the genitals of both women and men are viewed as ugly, misshapen and unappealing if left in their ‘natural’ state. For many African men and women the ideal of beauty is associated with a sexual anatomy that is smooth, cleansed and cleaned (shaved) and free of all ‘fleshy encumbrances.’

There is of course much more to be said about the beliefs, values, sanctions, rules, motives, and satisfactions associated with female (and male) genital surgeries in Africa. Nevertheless, in the light of these and other culturally endorsed reasons a genital surgery is experienced as an improvement of the body in many East and West African ethnic groups (Ahmadu 2000).

The cultural psychology of the culturally endorsed practices of any particular community is likely to result in a depiction of ‘other minds’ that is unsettling, astonishing, or at least surprising for those whose goals, values, and pictures of the world are characteristic of the cultural psychology of some other group. Radical divergence in the moral evaluation of particular ‘custom complexes,’ such as ‘circumcision,’ is itself an important topic for research in cultural psychology. Indeed, the cultural psychology of moral evaluation is currently an active research area. Moral concepts (e.g., human rights, justice as equality) privileged in some sections of society and in some regions of the world are not necessarily the moral concepts that are most salient and important in other sectors of society and regions of the world (Shweder et al. 1997).

6. Cultural Psychology: What It Is Not

The tenet of psychic pluralism and the emphasis on ‘goals, values, and pictures of the world’ as a source of psychological differences between cultural communities distinguishes cultural psychology from other fields of study (such as ‘cross-cultural psychology’ and ‘national character studies’) with which it should not be confused.

6.1 It Is Not Cross-Cultural Psychology

Research in cultural psychology proceeds on the assumption that psychological diversity is inherent in the human condition, and that culture and psyche are interdependent and ‘make each other up.’ It should be noted in passing that any theory of psychological pluralism would lack credibility if it staunchly denied the existence of any and all universals. Indeed, as should be obvious, cultural psychology presupposes many psychological universals (e.g., feelings, wants, goals, ideas of good and bad, of cause and effect, of part–whole relationships, etc.). However, the search for and the privileging of things that are uniform across all peoples is a project that goes under other names, for example, general psychology or perhaps even ‘cross-cultural psychology.’ (See Schweder 2000 for a discussion of cross-cultural psychology, partly recapitulated here.)

Some of the goals of cross-cultural psychology (which I am contrasting with cultural psychology) have been described by Segall et al. (1998). One goal is ‘to generate more nearly universal psychology, one that has pan-human validity’ and to attain ‘a univer-sally applicable psychological theory.’ A second closely related goal is to ‘keep peeling away at the onion skin of culture so as to reveal the psychic unity of mankind at its core.’ It is for that reason that cross-cultural psychology (not to be confused with cultural psychology) can be viewed as a vigilant cousin of general psychology; for they both share the same uniformitarian goals. Such goals give a distinctive character to cross-cultural psychology, and they help explain why all of the following kinds of activities are typical of research by cross-cultural psychologists and distinguish it from research by cultural psychologists.

Thus one of the aims of cross-cultural psychology is to determine the boundary conditions for generalizations generated in Western laboratories with Western (mostly college student) subjects, generalizations which, prior to critical examination by cross-cultural psychologists, have been presumptively interpreted as fundamental or ‘natural’ and universalized to the whole world. The aim here for the cross-cultural psychologist is not to represent the distinctive cultural psychology of particular peoples. This aim does not result in research focused on differences in the way members of different communities perceive, categorize, feel, want, choose, evaluate, and communicate that can be traced to differences in salient community-based ‘goals, values, and pictures of the world.’ Rather the aim for the cross-cultural psychologist is to make sure that the hoped-for universal psychology is truly universal and to throw out any claim that only holds in the Anglo-American world. This is an extremely useful corrective for the tendency of Western psychologists to overgeneralize their findings, but it is not the same as undertaking a project in cultural psychology.

A second aim of cross-cultural psychology is to establish comparability or equivalence for measuring instruments across different populations. Often the goal here is to try to show that people in different cultures really are alike, and that any reported differences in performance were due to noise, or inappropriate measuring instruments, or bad translations, or misunderstandings about the way to ask and answer questions. The instincts of a cultural psychologist run in quite a different direction. For a cultural psychologist (not to be confused with a ‘cross-cultural psychologist’) the ‘noise’ is interpreted as a signal about true differences in cultural meanings (goals, values, and pictures of the world), and not as some-thing to eliminate or overcome. Indeed, cultural psychologists are likely to worry if one’s measuring instruments travel easily and well from university classroom to university classroom around the world and display the same psychometric properties here and there. They may suspect that one has not really landed in a truly different culture at all. This is because ‘peeling away the onion skin of culture so as to reveal the psychic unity of mankind at its core’ is not what cultural psychology is about.

A third major aim of cross-cultural psychology is to focus on ‘independent variables’ of the cultural environment (for example, nucleation of the family, literacy vs. non-literacy) that are thought either to promote or to retard psychological development. In such research ‘development’ is almost always defined in terms of universal norms for cognitive, emotional, or social functioning (e.g., Piaget’s notion of ‘formal operational’ thinking or Ainsworth’s notion of healthy ‘attachment’). Cultural psychology is primarily concerned with the elaboration and discovery of alternative or plural norms for successful psychological development, which is another way it can be distinguished from cross-cultural psychology (LeVine 1990).

6.2 It Is Not National Character Studies

Attempts to characterize whole populations in terms of generalized dispositions (e.g., authoritarianism, Apollonianism, high need for achievement) went out of fashion in psychological anthropology in the late 1950s and early 1960s. This happened for several reasons but largely because looking for variations in types of personality traits to explain differences in cultural practices or custom complexes (and vice versa) turned out to be something of a dead end. It was discovered that if one tries to describe individuals within and across cultural communities in terms of general dispositions or traits of character, then within-group variations typically exceed between-group variations. It was discovered that hypothesized ‘modal personality’ types typically characterize only about a third of the population in any particular cultural group.

A major insight, although a fragile one, of recent work in cultural psychology is that it is better to represent and interpret human behavior the way sensible economists do rather than the way personality trait theorists do. That is to say, it is better to think about behavior as emanating from ‘agency,’ and to analyze it as the joint product of ‘preferences’ (including goals, values, and ‘ends’ of various sorts) and ‘constraints’ (including beliefs, information, skills, material and social resources, and ‘means’ of various sorts). This avoids the hazards of dispositional approaches in which behavior is interpreted as the by-product of mechanical forces pushing both from inside (in the form of ‘personality traits’) and outside (in the form of ‘situational pressures’). Ultimately, a fully successful piece of research in cultural psychology must avoid nominal dispositional categories such as ‘holistic vs. analytic’ and render behavior intelligible in terms of the particular goals, values, and pictures of the world that motivate and inform the ‘domain-specific’ behaviors and routine practices of specific intentional agents. To do otherwise is to reify cultural stereotypes and fall into some of the traps of the past.

7. The Future: Going Indigenous

The field of cultural psychology that has re-emerged on the North American and European scene during the past 20 years is quite similar to an intellectual movement that has grown up in the non-Western world and is increasingly known as ‘indigenous psychology.’ One of the most eminent theoreticians of this movement, Kuo-shu Yang, the Taiwanese psychologist, lists several ways to ‘indigenize’ psychological research. Here are four of Professor Yang’s virtues for the aspiring indigenous psychologist of China (Yang 1997):

(a) ‘Give priority to the study of culturally unique psychological and behavioral phenomena or characteristics of the Chinese people.’

(b) ‘Investigate both the specific content and the involved process of the phenomenon.’

(c) Make it a rule to begin any research with a thorough immersion into the natural, concrete details of the phenomenon to be studied.

(d) Let research be based upon the Chinese intellectual tradition rather than the Western intellectual tradition.

Those are some of the virtues that define ‘cultural psychology’ as well, although it remains to be seen how many of us can live up to such demanding standards. Even today with the rebirth and renewal of cultural psychology not all research actually begins with fieldwork or with ‘a thorough immersion into the natural concrete details of the phenomenon to be studied.’ All too often research still starts with a published finding from some Western laboratory, which is then subjected to critical examination by means of various attempts at replication with populations from other societies. But things are changing. Given the increasingly international and interdisciplinary character of collaborative scholarship in cultural psychology one looks forward to more and more research that keeps faith with Kuo-shu Yang’s high ideals.

Bibliography:

  1. Ahmadu F 2000 Rites and wrongs: An insider outsider reflects on power and excision. In: Shell-Duncan B, Hernlund Y (eds.) Female ‘Circumcision’ in Africa: Culture, Change and Controversy. Lynne Rienner, Boulder, CO, pp. 283–312
  2. Berlin I 1976 Vico and Herder. Hogarth, London
  3. Boesch E E 1991 Symbolic Action Theory and Cultural Psychology. Springer, Berlin
  4. Briggs J L 1970 Never in Anger: Portrait of an Eskimo Family. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
  5. Bruner J S 1990 Acts of Meaning. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
  6. Cole M 1996 Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
  7. D’Andrade R G 1995 The Development of Cognitive Anthropology. Cambridge University Press, New York
  8. Geertz C 1973 The Interpretation of Culture. Basic Books, New York
  9. Greenfield P, Cocking R 1994 Cross-cultural Roots of Minority Child Development. Erlbaum, Hindsdale, NJ
  10. Jahoda G 1991 Crossroads Between Culture and Mind: Continuities and Change in Theories of Human Nature. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
  11. Kitayama S, Markus H R 1994 Emotion and Culture: Empirical Studies of Mutual Influence. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC
  12. Kleinman A 1986 The Social Origins of Distress and Disease. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT
  13. LeVine R A 1990 Infant environments in psychoanalysis: A cross-cultural view. In: Stigler J, Shweder R, Herdt G (eds.) Cultural Psychology: Essays on Comparative Human Development. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 454–74
  14. Levy R 1973 Tahitians: Mind and Experience in the Society Islands. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
  15. Lutz C 1988 Unnatural Emotions: Everyday Sentiments on a Micronesian Atoll and Their Change to Western Theory. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
  16. Markus H R, Kitayama S 1991 Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review 98: 224–53
  17. Markus H R, Kitayama S, Heiman R 1996 Culture and ‘basic’ psychological principles. In: Higgins E T, Kruglanski A W (eds.) Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. Guilford, New York, pp. 857–913
  18. Menon U 2000 Does feminism have universal relevance? The challenges posed by Oriya Hindu family practices. Daedalus: Journal of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences 129: 77–100. [Special issue on The End of Tolerance: Engaging Cultural Differences]
  19. Menon U, Shweder R A 1998 The return of the ‘white man’s burden’: the encounter between the moral discourse of anthropology and the domestic life of Hindu women. In: Shweder R A (ed.) Welcome to Middle Age! (And Other Cultural Fictions). University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 139–88
  20. Miller J G 1997 Theoretical issues in cultural psychology. In: Triandis H C (ed.) Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology, 1: Theory and Method. Allyn and Bacon, Boston, MA, pp. 85–128
  21. Nisbett R E, Cohen D 1995 The Culture of Honor: The Psychology of Violence in the South. Westview, Boulder, CO Nisbett R E, Peng K, Choi I, Norenzayan A (in press). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychological Review
  22. Obermeyer C M 1999 Female genital surgeries: The known, the unknown, and the unknowable. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 13: 79–106
  23. Segall M H, Lonner W J, Berry J W 1998 Cross-cultural psychology as a scholarly discipline. American Psychologist 53: 1101–10
  24. Shore B 1996 Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture and the Problem of Meaning. Oxford University Press, New York
  25. Shweder R A 1991 Thinking Through Cultures: Expeditions in Cultural Psychology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
  26. Shweder R A 2000 The psychology of practice and the practice of the three psychologies. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 3: 207–21
  27. Shweder R A, Goodnow J, Hatano G, LeVine R, Markus H, Miller P 1998 The cultural psychology of development: One mind, many mentalities. In: Damon W (ed.) Handbook of Child Psychology, 1. John Wiley, New York, pp. 865–937
  28. Shweder R A, Haidt J 2000 The cultural psychology of the emotions. In: Lewis M, Haviland J (eds.) The Handbook of Emotions, 2nd edn., Guilford, New York, pp. 397–414
  29. Shweder R A, LeVine R A 1984 Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self and Emotion. Cambridge University Press, New York
  30. Shweder R A, Much N C, Mahapatra M, Park L 1997 The ‘big three’ of morality (autonomy, community, divinity), and the ‘big three’ explanations of suff In: Rozin P, Brandt A (eds.) Morality and Health. Routledge, New York, pp. 119–69
  31. Valsiner J (ed.) 1997 The legacy of Ernst E Boesch in cultural psychology. Culture & Psychology [Special Issue] 3: 243–7
  32. White G M, Kirkpatrick J 1985 Person, Self and Experience: Exploring Pacific Ethnopsychologies. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA
  33. Whiting J W M, Child I 1953 Child Training and Personality. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT
  34. Wierzbicka A 1993 A conceptual basis for cultural psychology. Ethos 21: 205–31
  35. Wierzbicka A 1999 Emotions Across Languages and Cultures: Diversity and Universals. Cambridge University Press, New York
  36. Yang, K 1997 Indigenizing Westernized Chinese psychology. In: Bond M H (ed.) Working at the Interface of Culture: Twenty Lives in Social Science. Routledge, London
Psychology Of Curiosity Research Paper
Cross-Cultural Psychology Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!