Mastery Learning Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

This sample education research paper on Mastery Learning features: 5500 words (approx. 18 pages) and a bibliography with 52 sources. Browse other research paper examples for more inspiration. If you need a thorough research paper written according to all the academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help. This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our writing service for professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable rates.

Over the last four decades, few programs have been implemented as broadly or evaluated as thoroughly as those associated with mastery learning. Programs based on mastery learning operate today in nations throughout the world and at every level of education. When compared to students in traditionally taught classes, students in mastery learning classes consistently have been shown to learn better, reach higher levels of achievement, and develop greater confidence in their ability to learn and in themselves as learners (Anderson, 1994; Guskey & Pigott, 1988; C. C. Kulik, Kulik, & Bangert-Drowns, 1990).

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


This research paper describes how mastery learning originated and the essential elements involved in its implementation. It discusses the improvements in student learning that typically result from the use of mastery learning and how this strategy provides practical solutions to a variety of pressing instructional problems. Finally it explains the common misinterpretations of mastery learning and summarizes the results of research on its effects.

John B. Carroll’s Model for School Learning

Although the basic tenets of mastery learning can be traced to such early educators as Comenius, Pestalozzi, and Herbart, a major influence on the development of modern versions was a 1963 article by Harvard University professor John B. Carroll entitled, “A Model for School Learning.” In this article, Carroll challenged long-held notions about student aptitude. He pointed out that student aptitude traditionally had been viewed as the level to which a child could learn a particular subject. Children with high aptitude would be able to learn the most complex aspects of that subject, while those with low aptitude would be able to learn only the most basic elements. When aptitude is viewed in this way, children are seen as either good learners (high aptitude) or poor learners (low aptitude) with regard to the subject.




Carroll argued, however, that student aptitude more accurately reflects an index of learning rate. That is, all children have the potential to learn quite well but differ primarily in terms of the time they require to do so. Some children are able to learn a subject very quickly while others may take much longer. When aptitude is viewed as an index of learning rate, children are seen not simply as good and poor learners but rather as fast and slow learners.

Carroll then proposed a model for school learning based on this alternative view of aptitude. He believed that if each child were allowed the time needed to learn a subject to some criterion level, and if the child spent that time appropriately, then the child probably would attain the specified level of achievement. But if not enough time were allowed or if the child did not spend the time required, then the child would learn much less. The degree of learning attained by a child, therefore, can be expressed by the following simple equation:

Degree of learning f = (time spent/ time needed)

In other words, degree of learning is a function of the time a child actually spends on learning relative to the time he or she needs to spend. If the time spent were equal to time needed, the learning would be complete and the equation would equal 1. If the time spent were less than the time needed, however, the learning would be incomplete by that proportion.

Carroll further identified the factors that he believed influenced the time spent and the time needed. He argued that both of these elements were affected by characteristics of the learner and by characteristics of the instruction. Specifically, he believed that the time spent was determined by a child’s perseverance and the opportunity to learn. Perseverance is simply the amount of time a child is willing to spend actively engaged in learning. Opportunity to learn is the classroom time allotted to the learning. Time spent is determined by the child’s persistence at a learning task and the amount of learning time provided. Time needed, on the other hand, Carroll believed was determined by the child’s learning rate for that subject, the quality of the instruction, and the child’s ability to understand the instruction, specifically represented by the following equation:

Degree of learning f =(perseverance, opportunity to learn\ learning rate, quality of instruction, ability to understand the instruction)

Again, a child’s learning rate is a measure of the time required by the child to learn the concepts or skills under ideal instructional conditions. If the quality of the instruction were high, then the child would readily understand it and would probably need little time to learn. If the quality of the instruction were not as high, however, then the child would have greater difficulty understanding and would require much more time to learn. The quality of the instruction and the child’s ability to understand the instruction interact to determine how much time is needed for the child to learn the concepts or skills.

Carroll’s article made a significant contribution to learning theory. It set forth new guidelines for research into the concept of aptitude and identified specific factors that influence learning in school settings. His ideas about learning rate also prompted the development of numerous individualized instruction programs that allowed students to progress through a series of learning units at their own, self-determined pace. Two of the best known of these continuous progress programs were Individually Prescribed Instruction (IPI), developed at the University of Pittsburgh (Glaser, 1966), and Individually Guided Education (IGE), developed at the University of Wisconsin (Klausmeier, Rossmiller, & Saily, 1977). Carroll himself, however, did not address the problem of how to provide sufficient time or how to improve instructional quality. These issues were left unresolved.

Benjamin S. Bloom’s Learning for Mastery

During the 1960s, Benjamin S. Bloom and his students at the University of Chicago were deeply involved in research on individual differences and ways to improve the teaching and learning process. Bloom was impressed by the optimism of Carroll’s perspective on learners and particularly by the idea that students differ in terms of the time required for learning rather than their ability to learn. If aptitude were indeed predictive of the time a child would require to learn, Bloom believed it should be possible to set the degree of learning expected of each child at some mastery performance level. Then by attending to the instructional variables under teachers’ control—the opportunity to learn and the quality of the instruction—teachers should be able to ensure that every child attains that specified level. Bloom believed that if sufficient time and appropriate instruction were provided virtually all students could learn.

To determine how this might be practically achieved, Bloom first considered how teaching and learning take place in typical group-based classroom settings. He observed that most teachers begin by dividing the concepts and skills that they want students to learn into smaller learning units. These units are usually sequentially ordered and often correspond to the research papers in the textbook used. Teachers then teach the unit concepts to all students in the same way, provide all students with the same amount of time to learn, and evaluate students’ learning at the end with some form of test or assessment. The few students for whom the instructional methods and time were ideal learn excellently and perform well on the unit assessment. The largest number of students, for whom the methods and time were only moderately appropriate, learn less well. And students for whom the instruction and time were inappropriate due to differences in their backgrounds or learning styles learn very little and perform poorly on the unit assessment. Little variation in the teaching resulted in great variation in student learning. Under these conditions the pattern of student achievement was similar to the normal curve distribution shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1      Distribution of Achievement in Traditional Classrooms

Mastery Learning Research Paper

 

To attain better results and reduce this variation in student achievement, Bloom reasoned that we would have to increase variation in the teaching. That is, because students vary in their learning styles and aptitudes, teachers must diversify and differentiate instruction to better meet students’ individual learning needs. The challenge was to find practical ways to do this within the constraints of group-based classrooms so that all students could learn well.

In searching for such a strategy, Bloom drew primarily from two sources of evidence. First, he considered the ideal teaching and learning situation in which an excellent tutor is paired with each student. He was particularly influenced by the work of early pioneers in individualized instruction, especially Washburne (1922) and his Winnetka Plan, and Morrison (1926) and his University of Chicago Laboratory School experiments. In examining this evidence, Bloom tried to determine what crucial elements in one-on-one tutoring and individualized instruction could be transferred to group-based classroom settings.

Second, Bloom looked at studies of the learning strategies of academically successful students, especially the work of Dollard and Miller (1950). From this research he tried to identify the activities of high-achieving students in group-based classrooms that distinguish them from their less successful classmates.

Bloom saw value in teachers’ traditional practice of organizing the concepts and skills they want students to learn into learning units. He also thought it important for teachers to assess student learning at the end of each unit. But the classroom assessments most teachers used seemed to do little more than show for whom their initial instruction was or was not appropriate.

Bloom believed that a far better approach would be for teachers to use their classroom assessments as learning tools, and then to follow those assessments with a feedback and corrective procedure. Instead of using assessments only as evaluation devices that mark the end of each unit, Bloom recommended using them as part of the instructional process to diagnose individual learning difficulties (feedback) and to prescribe remediation procedures (correctives).

This is precisely what takes place when an excellent tutor works with an individual student. If the student makes an error, the tutor first points out the error (feedback) and then follows up with further explanation and clarification (correctives) to ensure the student’s understanding. Similarly, academically successful students typically follow up the mistakes they make on quizzes and assessments. They ask the teacher about the items they missed, look up the answer in the textbook or other resources, or rework the problem or task so that they do not repeat those errors.

With this in mind, Bloom outlined an instructional strategy to make use of this feedback and corrective procedure, labeling it “Learning for Mastery” (Bloom, 1968) and later shortening it to simply “Mastery Learning” (Bloom, 1971a). To use this strategy, teachers first organize the concepts and skills they want students to learn into learning units that typically involve about a week or two of instructional time. Following initial instruction on the unit, teachers administer a brief quiz or assessment based on the unit’s learning goals. Instead of signifying the end of the unit, however, this assessment’s purpose is to give students information, or feedback, about their learning. To emphasize this new purpose, Bloom suggested calling it a formative assessment, meaning “to inform or provide information.” A formative assessment identifies for students precisely what they have learned well to that point and what they need to learn better (Bloom, Hastings, & Madaus, 1971).

Paired with each formative assessment are specific corrective activities for students to use in correcting their learning difficulties. Most teachers match these correctives to each item or set of prompts within the assessment so that students need to work on only those concepts or skills not yet mastered. In other words, the correctives are individualized. Correctives may point out additional sources of information on a particular concept, such as page numbers in the textbook or workbook where the concept is discussed. They may identify alternative learning resources such as different textbooks, learning kits, alternative materials, CDs, videos, or Web-based instructional lessons. Or they may simply suggest sources of additional practice, such as study guides, computer exercises, independent or guided practice activities, or collaborative group activities.

With the feedback and corrective information gained from the formative assessment, each student has a detailed prescription of what more needs to be done to master the concepts or skills from the unit. This just-in-time correction prevents minor learning difficulties from accumulating and becoming major learning problems. It also gives teachers a practical means to vary and differentiate their instruction in order to better meet students’ individual learning needs. As a result, many more students learn well, master the important learning goals in each unit, and gain the necessary prerequisites for success in subsequent units.

When students complete their corrective activities after a class period or two, Bloom recommended they take a second formative assessment. This second, parallel assessment covers the same concepts and skills as the first, but is composed of slightly different problems or questions and serves two important purposes. First, it verifies whether the correctives were successful in helping students overcome their individual learning difficulties. Second, it offers students a second chance at success and, hence, has powerful motivational value.

Some students, of course, will perform well on the first assessment, demonstrating that they have mastered the unit concepts and skills. The teacher’s initial instruction was highly appropriate for these students, and they have no need for corrective work. To ensure continued learning progress for these students, Bloom recommended that teachers provide them with special enrichment or extension activities to broaden their learning experiences. Enrichment activities are often self-selected by students and might involve special projects or reports, academic  games, or a variety of complex, problem-solving tasks. Figure 2 illustrates this instructional sequence.

Figure 2  The Mastery Learning Instructional Process

Mastery Learning Research Paper

Bloom believed all students could receive a more appropriate quality of instruction through this process than is possible under more traditional approaches to teaching. As a result, nearly all students might be expected to learn well and truly master the unit concepts or learning goals (Bloom, 1976). This, in turn, would drastically reduce the variation in students’ achievement levels, eliminate achievement gaps, and yield a distribution of achievement more like that shown in Figure 3.

In describing mastery learning, Bloom emphasized that reducing variation in students’ achievement does not imply making all students the same. Even under these more favorable learning conditions, some students undoubtedly will learn more than others, especially those involved in enrichment activities. But Bloom believed the variation among students in how well they learn specific concepts or master a set of articulated learning goals could eventually reach a “vanishing point” if teachers recognized relevant, individual differences among students and then altered instruction to better meet their diverse learning needs (Bloom, 1971b). In other words, all students could learn well the knowledge and skills prescribed in the curriculum. As a result, gaps in the achievement of different groups of students would be closed.

Figure 3  Distribution of Achievement in Mastery Learning Classrooms

Mastery Learning Research Paper

The Essential Elements of Mastery Learning

After Benjamin Bloom described his ideas, several of his students took up the task of clarifying mastery learning instructional strategies (e.g., Block, Efthim, & Burns, 1989), and numerous programs based on mastery learning principles sprung up in schools throughout the United States and around the world (see Postlethwaite & Hag-garty, 1998; Reezigt & Weide, 1990; Wu, 1994; Yildiran, 2006). Although differing from setting to setting, those programs true to Bloom’s ideas include two essential elements: the feedback, corrective, and enrichment process, and (2) instructional alignment (Guskey, 1997a).

Feedback, Correctives, and Enrichment

Teachers who use mastery learning provide students with frequent and specific feedback on their learning progress through regular, formative classroom assessments. This feedback is both diagnostic and prescriptive. It reinforces precisely what students were expected to learn, identifies what they learned well, and describes what needs to be learned better. The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM) emphasizes this same element in its latest iteration of the standards for school mathematics. To overcome inequities in mathematics instruction, NCTM stresses the use of assessments that support learning and provide useful information to both teachers and students (National Council of Teachers of Mathematics [NCTM], 2000).

Feedback alone, however, does little to help students improve their learning. Significant improvement requires that feedback be paired with correctives: activities that offer guidance and direction to students on how to remedy their learning problems. Because of students’ individual differences, no single method of instruction works best for all. To help every student learn well, therefore, teachers must differentiate their instruction, both in their initial teaching and especially through the corrective activities (Bloom, 1976). In other words, teachers must increase variation in their teaching to decrease variation in results.

To be effective, correctives must be qualitatively different from the initial teaching. They must provide students who need it with an alternative approach and additional time to learn. The best correctives present concepts differently and involve students in learning differently than did the initial instruction. They incorporate different learning styles, learning modalities, or types of intelligence. Although developing effective correctives can prove challenging, many schools find that providing teachers with time to work collaboratively—sharing ideas, materials, and expertise—greatly facilitates the process (Guskey, 2001).

Most applications of mastery learning also include enrichment or extension activities for students who master the unit concepts from the initial instruction. As described earlier, enrichment activities offer students exciting opportunities to broaden and expand their learning. They reward students for their learning success and challenge them to go further. Many teachers draw from activities developed for gifted and talented students when planning enrichment activities, both to simplify implementation tasks and to guarantee these students a high-quality learning experience.

Teachers implement the feedback, corrective, and enrichment process in a variety of ways. Many use short, paper-and-pencil quizzes as formative assessments to give students feedback on their learning progress. But formative assessments also can take the form of essays, compositions, projects, reports, performance tasks, skill demonstrations, oral presentations, or any device used to gain evidence on students’ learning progress. In essence, teachers adapt the format of their formative assessments to match their instructional goals.

After administering a formative assessment, some teachers divide the class into separate corrective and enrichment groups. While the teacher directs corrective activities, guaranteeing that all students who need the extra time and assistance take part, the other students work on self-selected, independent enrichment activities. Other teachers pair with colleagues and use a team-teaching approach. While one teacher oversees corrective activities, the other monitors enrichments. Still other teachers use cooperative learning activities in which students work together in teams to ensure all reach the mastery level. Since students have their own personal scores on the formative assessment, individual accountability is ensured. Offering the entire team special recognition or credit if all members attain mastery on the second formative assessment encourages group responsibility.

Feedback, corrective, and enrichment procedures are crucial to mastery learning, for it is through these procedures that mastery learning differentiates and individualizes instruction. In every learning unit, students who need extended time and opportunity to remedy learning problems receive these through the correctives. Students who learn quickly and find the initial instruction highly appropriate have opportunities to extend their learning through enrichment. As a result, all students experience more favorable learning conditions and more appropriate, higher quality instruction (Bloom, 1977).

Instructional Alignment

While feedback, correctives, and enrichment are important, they alone do not constitute mastery learning. Bloom stressed that to be truly effective they must be combined with the second essential element of mastery learning: instructional alignment. Reducing variation in student learning and closing achievement gaps requires clarity and consistency among all instructional components (Bloom, 1971a).

Bloom believed three major components composed the teaching and learning process. To begin there must be specific ideas about what students are expected to learn and be able to do—that is, learning goals or standards. Next comes instruction that ideally results in proficient learners—students who have learned well and whose proficiency can be assessed through some form of assessment or evaluation. Mastery learning adds a feedback and corrective component, allowing teachers to determine for whom their initial instruction was appropriate and for whom an alternative approach may be needed.

Although essentially neutral with regard to what is taught, how it is taught, and how learning is assessed or evaluated, mastery learning requires consistency or alignment among these instructional components, as shown in Figure 4. For example, if students are expected to learn higher-level skills such as those involved in making applications, solving complex problems, or developing thoughtful analyses, mastery learning stipulates that instructional activities must be planned to give students opportunities to practice and actively engage in those skills. It also requires that students be given specific feedback on how well they have learned those skills, coupled with directions on how to correct any learning errors. Finally, procedures for assessing or evaluating students’ learning should reflect those higher-level skills as well.

Figure 4      Major Components in the Teaching and Learning Process

Mastery Learning Research Paper

To ensure alignment among instructional components, teachers must make a number of crucial decisions. First, they need to decide what concepts or skills are most important for students to learn and most central to students’ understanding. Teachers must determine, for example, if they want students to learn only basic skills or if they want students to develop higher-level skills and more complex cognitive processes. Second, teachers need to decide what evidence best reflects students’ mastery of those basic or

higher-level skills. Critics sometimes challenge teachers’ abilities to make these crucial decisions. But, in essence, teachers at all levels already make these decisions in conducting regular classroom activities. Every time they administer an assessment, grade a paper, or evaluate students’ learning, teachers communicate to students what is most important to learn. Using mastery learning simply compels teachers to make these decisions more thoughtfully, intentionally, and purposefully.

Misinterpretations of Mastery Learning

Shortly after Bloom presented his ideas on mastery learning, people began to misinterpret them. Some early attempts to apply mastery learning, for example, were based on narrow and inaccurate understandings of Bloom’s theory. These efforts focused only on low-level cognitive skills, attempted to break down learning into small segments, and insisted that students master each segment before being permitted to move on. Teachers were regarded in these programs as little more than managers of materials and record-keepers of student progress. Unfortunately, similar misinterpretations of mastery learning persist.

Nowhere in Bloom’s writing, however, can this kind of narrowness and rigidity be found. In fact, Bloom emphasized quite the opposite. He considered thoughtful and reflective teachers vital to the successful implementation of mastery learning and continually stressed flexibility in its application. In his earliest description of the process, Bloom wrote:

There are many alternative strategies for mastery learning. Each strategy must find some way of dealing with individual differences in learners through some means of relating the instruction to the needs and characteristics of the learners. . . . The nongraded school is one attempt to provide an organizational structure that permits and encourages mastery learning. (1968, pp. 7-8)

Bloom further emphasized his belief that instruction in mastery learning classrooms should focus on higher-level learning goals, not simply basic skills. He noted:

I find  great  emphasis on problem solving, applications of principles, analytical skills, and creativity. Such higher mental processes are emphasized because this type of learning enables the individual to relate his or her learning to the many problems he or she encounters in day-to-day living. These abilities are stressed because they are retained and utilized long after the individual has forgotten the detailed specifics of the subject matter taught in the schools. These abilities are regarded as one set of essential characteristics needed to continue learning and to cope with a rapidly changing world. (Bloom, 1978, p. 576)

Modern research studies have shown mastery learning to be particularly effective when applied to instruction focusing on higher-level learning goals such as problem solving, drawing inferences, deductive reasoning, and creative expression (Arredondo & Block, 1990; Kozlovsky, 1990; Mevarech, 1981). When well implemented, the process helps teachers improve student learning and close achievement gaps in a broad range of learning goals from basic skills to highly complex cognitive processes.

Another misinterpretation comes from educators who believe that the constraint of limited class time will inhibit efforts to implement mastery learning (Guskey, 1997a). They assume that the introduction of feedback, corrective, and enrichment procedures will reduce the amount of material teachers will be able to cover. Teachers fear they will have to sacrifice coverage for the sake of mastery. Minor alterations in instructional pacing typically resolve this concern.

Early mastery learning units usually do require more time. Teachers who allot class time for students to complete corrective activities often find themselves behind other teachers who teach in more traditional ways after the first two or three units. But once students become familiar with the process, mastery learning teachers generally pick up the pace. Because students in mastery learning classes spend a larger portion of their time actively engaged in learning, they make more rapid progress than students in traditionally taught classes (Fitzpatrick, 1985). As students catch on to the process, they also do better on first formative assessments. With fewer students involved in correctives and less corrective work needed, teachers reduce the class time allocated to corrective activities. And because mastery learning students learn well the concepts and skills from early units, they are better prepared for later, more advanced units. Instruction in later units can therefore be more rapid and include fewer review activities. Most teachers discover that with slight adjustments in the pacing of their instruction—slightly more time spent in early units but less time in later ones—they can cover just as much material using mastery learning, and in some cases more, as they were able to using more traditional approaches to instruction (Block, 1983).

Research Results and Implications

Teachers who implement mastery learning generally find that it requires only modest changes in their instructional procedures. Excellent teachers use many aspects of mastery learning in their classes already, and others discover that the process blends well with their current teaching strategies. This makes mastery learning particularly attractive to teachers at all levels, especially considering the difficulties associated with new approaches that require major changes in teaching procedures.

Despite the modest nature of these changes, however, extensive research evidence shows that the use of mastery learning can have exceptionally positive effects on student learning (Block et al., 1989; Guskey & Pigott, 1988). Evidence gathered in Asia (Wu, 1994), Australia (Chan, 1981), Europe (Langeheine, 1992; Mevarech, 1981; Reezigt & Weide, 1990), and the United States (Walberg, 1990) shows the careful and systematic application of these elements can lead to significant improvements in student learning outcomes. Some researchers even suggest that the superiority of Japanese students in international comparisons of achievement in mathematics operations and problem solving may be due largely to the widespread use of instructional practices similar to mastery learning in Japan (Nakajima, 2006; Waddington, 1995).

Long-term investigations have yielded similarly impressive results. A study by Whiting, Van Burgh, and Render (1995), representing 18 years of data gathered from over 7,000 high school students, showed mastery learning to have remarkably positive influence on students’ test scores and grade point averages as well as their attitudes toward school and learning. Another field experiment conducted in elementary and middle school classrooms showed that the implementation of mastery learning led to significantly positive increases in students’ academic achievement and their self-confidence (Anderson et al., 1992). Even more impressive, a comprehensive, meta-analysis review of the research on mastery learning by C. C. Kulik et al. (1990) concluded:

We recently reviewed meta-analyses in nearly 40 different areas of educational research (J. Kulik & Kulik, 1989). Few educational treatments of any sort were consistently associated with achievement effects as large as those produced by mastery learning. . . . In evaluation after evaluation, mastery programs have produced impressive gains. (C. C. Kulik et al., 1990, p. 292)

Research evidence also shows that the positive effects of mastery learning are not limited to cognitive or achievement outcomes. The process also yields improvements in students’ confidence in learning situations, school attendance rates, involvement in class sessions, attitudes toward learning, and a variety of other affective measures (Block et al., 1989; Guskey & Pigott, 1988; Whiting & Render, 1987). This multidimensional impact has been referred to as the “multiplier effect” of mastery learning and makes it one of the most cost-effective means of educational improvement.

It should be noted that one review of the research on mastery learning, contrary to all previous reviews, indicated that the process had essentially no effect on student achievement (Slavin, 1987). This finding surprised not only scholars familiar with the vast research literature on mastery learning showing it to yield very positive results, but also large numbers of practitioners who had experienced its positive effects firsthand. A close inspection of this review shows, however, that it was conducted using techniques of questionable validity (Hiebert, 1987), employed capricious selection criteria (J. A. Kulik, Kulik, & Bangert-Drowns, 1990), reported results in a biased manner (Bloom, 1987; Walberg, 1988), and drew conclusions not substantiated by the evidence presented (Guskey, 1987, 1988). Two much more extensive and methodologically sound reviews published since (Guskey & Pigott, 1988; C. C. Kulik et al., 1990) have verified mastery learning’s consistently positive effect on a broad range of student learning outcomes and, in one case (i.e., J. A. Kulik et al., 1990), showed clearly the distorted nature of this earlier report.

Conclusion

Researchers today generally recognize the value of the essential elements of mastery learning and their importance in effective teaching at any level of education. As a result, fewer studies are being conducted on the mastery learning process per se. Instead, researchers are looking for ways to enhance results further, adding additional elements to the mastery learning process that positively contribute to student learning in hopes of attaining even more impressive gains (Bloom, 1984). Work on the inte-gration of mastery learning with other innovative strategies appears especially promising (Guskey, 1997b).

In his later writing Bloom, too, described exciting work on other ideas designed to attain results even more positive than those typically achieved with mastery learning (Bloom, 1984). These ideas stemmed from the work of two of Bloom’s doctoral students, Anania (1981) and Burke (1983), who compared student learning under three different instructional conditions. The first was conventional instruction in which students were taught in group-based classes that included about 30 students and in which periodic assessments were given mainly for the purposes of grading. The second was mastery learning, in which students again were taught in group-based classes of about 30 students but were administered regular formative assessments for feedback, followed by individualized corrective instruction and parallel second assessments to determine the extent to which they mastered specific learning goals. The third was tutoring, where a good tutor was paired with each student. Students under tutoring were also administered regular formative assessments, along with corrective procedures and parallel second assessments, although the need for corrective work under tutoring was usually quite small.

The differences in students’ final achievement under these three conditions were striking. Using the standard deviation (sigma) of the control (conventional) class as the measure of difference, Bloom’s students discovered that:

The average student under tutoring was about two standard deviations above the average of the control class (the average tutored student was above 98% of the students in the control class). The average student under mastery learning was about one standard deviation above the average of the control class (the average mastery learning student was above 84% of the students in the control class). . . . Thus under the best learning conditions we can devise (tutoring), the average student is 2 sigma above the average control student taught under conventional group methods of instruction. (Bloom, 1984, p. 4)

Bloom referred to this as the “2 Sigma Problem”:

The tutoring process demonstrates that most students do have the potential to reach this high level of learning. I believe an important task of research and instruction is to seek ways of accomplishing this under more practical and realistic conditions than the one-to-one tutoring, which is too costly for most societies to bear on a large scale. This is the “2 Sigma” problem. Can researchers and teachers devise teaching-learning conditions that will enable the majority of students under group instruction to attain levels of achievement that can at present be reached only under tutoring conditions? (Bloom, 1984, pp. 4-5)

Bloom believed that attaining this high level of achievement would probably require more than just improvements in the quality of group instruction. Researchers and teachers might also need to find ways of improving students’ learning processes, the curriculum and instructional materials, the home environmental support of students’ school learning, and providing a focus on higher-level thinking skills. Nevertheless, Bloom remained convinced that careful attention to the elements of mastery learning would allow educators at all levels to make great strides in their efforts to reduce the variation in student achievement, close achievement gaps, and help all children to learn excellently.

Bibliography:

  1. Anania, J. (1981). The effects of quality of instruction on the cognitive and affective learning of students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.
  2. Anderson, S., Barrett, C., Huston, M., Lay, L., Myr, G., Sexton, D., et al. (1992). A mastery learning experiment (Technical Report). Yale, MI: Yale Public Schools.
  3. Anderson, S. A. (1994). Synthesis of research on mastery learning. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 382 567).
  4. Arredondo, D., & Block, J. (1990). Recognizing the connections between thinking skills and mastery learning. Educational Leadership, 47(5), 4-10.
  5. Block, J. H. (1983). Learning rates and mastery learning. Outcomes, 2(3), 18-23.
  6. Block, J. H., Efthim, H. E., & Burns, R. B. (1989). Building effective mastery learning schools. New York: Longman.
  7. Bloom, B. S. (1968). Learning for mastery. Evaluation Comment, 1(2), 1-12.
  8. Bloom, B. S. (1971a). Mastery learning. In J. H. Block (Ed.), Mastery learning: Theory and practice (pp. 47-63). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  9. Bloom, B. S. (1971b). Individual differences in school achievement: A vanishing point? Bloomington, IN: Phi Delta Kappan International.
  10. Bloom, B. S. (1976). Human characteristics and school learning. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  11. Bloom, B. S. (1977). Favorable learning conditions for all. Teacher, 95(3), 22-28.
  12. Bloom, B. S. (1978). New views of the learner: Implications for instruction and curriculum. Educational Leadership, 35(7), 563-576.
  13. Bloom, B. S. (1984). The 2 sigma problem: The search for methods of group instruction as effective as one-to-one tutoring. Educational Researcher, 13(6), 4-16.
  14. Bloom, B. S. (1987). A response to Slavin’s mastery learning reconsidered. Review of Educational Research, 57(4), 507-508.
  15. Bloom, B. S. (1988). Helping all children learn in elementary school and beyond. Principal, 67(4), 12-17.
  16. Bloom, B. S., Hastings, J. T., & Madaus, G. (1971). Handbook on formative and summative evaluation of student learning. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  17. Burke, A. J. (1983). Students’ potential for learning contrasted under tutorial and group approaches to instruction. Unpub-lished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.
  18. Carroll, J. B. (1963). A model for school learning. Teachers College Record, 64, 723-733.
  19. Chan, K. S. (1981). The interaction of aptitude with mastery versus non-mastery instruction: Effects on reading comprehension of grade three students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Western Australia.
  20. Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality andpsychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  21. Fitzpatrick, K. A. (1985, April). Group-based mastery learning: A Robin Hood approach to instruction? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.
  22. Glaser, R. (1966). The program for individually prescribed instruction. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh.
  23. Guskey, T. R. (1987). Rethinking mastery learning reconsidered. Review of Educational Research, 57(2), 225-229.
  24. Guskey, T. R. (1988). Response to Slavin: Who defines best? Educational Leadership, 46(2), 26-27.
  25. Guskey, T. R. (1997a). Implementing mastery learning (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
  26. Guskey, T. R. (1997b). Putting it all together: Integrating educational innovations. In S. J. Caldwell (Ed.), Professional
  27. Development in Learning-Centered Schools (pp. 130-149). Oxford, OH: National Staff Development Council.
  28. Guskey, T. R. (2001). Mastery learning. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 9372-9377). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science Ltd.
  29. Guskey, T. R., & Pigott, T. D. (1988). Research on group-based mastery learning programs: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Research, 81, 197-216.
  30. Hiebert, E. H. (1987). The context of instruction and student learning: An examination of Slavin’s assumptions. Review of Educational Research, 57, 337-340.
  31. Klausmeier, H. J., Rossmiller, R. A., & Saily, M. (1977). Individually guided elementary education: Concepts and practices. New York: Academic Press.
  32. Kozlovsky, J. D. (1990). Integrating thinking skills and mastery learning in Baltimore County. Educational Leadership, 47(5), 6.
  33. Kulik, C. C., Kulik, J. A., & Bangert-Drowns, R. L. (1990). Effectiveness of mastery learning programs: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 60, 265-299.
  34. Kulik, J. A., & Kulik, C. C. (1989). Meta-analysis in education.International Journal of Educational Research, 13(2), 221-340.
  35. Kulik, J. A., Kulik, C. C., & Bangert-Drowns, R. L. (1990). Is there better evidence on mastery learning? A response to Slavin. Review of Educational Research, 60, 303-307.
  36. Langeheine, R. (1992, April). State mastery learning: Dynamic models for longitudinal data. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.
  37. Levine, D. U. (1985). Improving student achievement through mastery learning programs. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
  38. Mevarech, Z. R. (1981, April). Attaining mastery on higher cognitive achievement. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Los Angeles.
  39. Morrison, H. C. (1926). The practice of teaching in the secondary school. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  40. Nakajima, A. (2006). A powerful influence on Japanese education. In T. R. Guskey (Ed.), Benjamin S. Bloom: Portraits of an educator (pp. 109-111). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Education.
  41. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author. Retrieved  from https://www.nctm.org/uploadedFiles/Standards_and_Positions/PSSM_ExecutiveSummary.pdf
  42. Postlethwaite, K., & Haggarty, L. (1998). Towards effective and transferable learning in secondary school: The development of an approach based on mastery learning. British Educational Research Journal, 24(3), 333-353.
  43. Reezigt, B. J., & Weide, M. G. (1990, April). The effects of group-based mastery learning on language and arithmetic achievement and attitudes in primary education in the Netherlands. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Boston.
  44. Slavin, R. E. (1987). Mastery learning reconsidered. Review of Educational Research, 57(2), 175-213.
  45. Waddington, T. (1995, April). Why mastery matters. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.
  46. Walberg, H. J. (1988). Response to Slavin: What’s the best evidence? Educational Leadership, 46(2), 28.
  47. Walberg, H. J. (1990). Productive teaching and instruction: Assessing the knowledge base. Phi Delta Kappan, 71, 470-478.
  48. Washburne, C. W. (1922). Educational measurements as a key to individualizing instruction and promotions. Journal of Educational Research, 5(1), 195-206.
  49. Whiting, B., & Render, G. F. (1987). Cognitive and affective outcomes of mastery learning: A review of sixteen semesters. The Clearing House, 60(6), 276-280.
  50. Whiting, B., Van Burgh, J. W., & Render, G. F. (1995). Mastery learning in the classroom. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.
  51. Wu, W. Y. (1994, April). Mastery learning in Hong Kong: Challenges and prospects. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
  52. Yildiran, G. (2006). Multicultural applications of mastery learning. Istanbul, Turkey: Faculty of Education, Bogazici University.
Plaget and Vygotsky Research Paper
Conceptual Learning Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!