Classical Test Theory Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

Sample Classical Test Theory Research Paper. Browse other research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. iResearchNet offers academic assignment help for students all over the world: writing from scratch, editing, proofreading, problem solving, from essays to dissertations, from humanities to STEM. We offer full confidentiality, safe payment, originality, and money-back guarantee. Secure your academic success with our risk-free services.

1. Introduction

One of the most striking and challenging phenomena in the Social Sciences is the unreliability of its measurements: Measuring the same attribute twice often yields two different results. If the same measurement instrument is applied twice, such a difference may sometimes be due to a change in the measured attribute itself. Sometimes these changes in the measured attribute are due to the mere fact of measuring. For example, people learn when solving tasks and they change their attitude when they reflect on statements in an attitude questionnaire. In other cases the change of the measured attribute is due to developmental phenomena, or it might be due to learning between occasions of measurement. However, if change of the attribute can be excluded two different results in measuring the same attribute can be explained only by ‘measurement error.’

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


Classical (Psychometric) Test Theory (CTT) aims at studying the reliability of a (real-valued) test score variable (measurement, test) that maps a crucial aspect of qualitative or quantitative observations into the set of real numbers. Aside from determining the reliability of a test score variable itself, CTT allows answering questions such as:

(a) How do two random variables correlate once the measurement error is filtered out (correction for attenuation)?




(b) How dependable is a measurement in characterizing an attribute of an individual unit, i.e., which is the confidence interval for the true score of that individual with respect to the measurement considered?

(c) How reliable is an aggregated measurement consisting of the average (or sum) of several measurements of the same unit or object (Spearman–Brown formula for test length)?

(d) How reliable is a difference, e.g., between a pretest and post-test?

2. Basic Concepts Of Classical Test Theory

2.1 Primitives

In the framework of CTT, each measurement (test score) is considered being a value of a random variable Y consisting of two components: a ‘true score’ and an ‘error score.’ Two levels, or more precisely, two random experiments may be distinguished: (a) sampling an observational unit (e.g., a person) and (b) sampling a score within a given unit. Within a given unit, the true score is a parameter, i.e., a given but unknown number characterizing the attribute of the unit, whereas the error is a random variable with an unknown distribution. The true score of the unit is defined to be the expectation of this intraindividual distribution.

Taking the across units perspective, i.e., joining the two random experiments, the true score is itself considered to be a value of a random variable (the ‘true score variable’). The ‘error variable’ is again a random variable, the distribution of which is a mixture of the individual units’ error distributions. Most theorems of CTT (e.g., Lord and Novick, 1968) are formulated from this across units’ perspective allowing talking about the correlation of true scores with other variables, for instance.

More formally, CTT refers to a ( joint) random experiment of (a) sampling an observational unit u (such as a person) from a set ΩU of units (called the population), and (b) registering one or more observations out of a set ΩO of possible observations. The set of possible outcomes of the random experiment is the set product:Ω=ΩU ×ΩO. The elements of ΩO, the observations, might be qualitative (such as ‘answering in category a of item 1 and in category b of item 2’), quantitative (such as reaction time and alcohol concentration in the blood), or consisting of both qualitative and quantitative components. In Psychology, the measurements are often defined by test scoring rules prescribing how the observations are transformed into test scores. (Hence, these measurement are also often called ‘tests’ or ‘test score variables.’) These scoring rules may just consist of summing initial scores of items (defining a psychological scale) or might be more sophisticated representations of observable attributes of the units. CTT does not prescribe the definition of the test score variables. It just additively decomposes them into true score variables and error variables. Substantive theory and empirical validation studies are necessary in order to decide whether or not a given test score variable is meaningful. CTT only helps disentangling the variances of its true score and error components.

Referring to the joint random experiment described above the mapping U: Ω→ ΩU, U(ω)= u, (the unit or person projection) may be considered a qualitative random variable having a joint distribution with the test scores variables Yi. Most theorems of CTT deal with two or more test score variables (tests) Yi and the relationship between their true score and error components. (The index i refer to one of several tests considered.)

2.2 The Core Concepts: True Score And Error Variables

Using the primitives introduced above, the true score variable τi:= E(Yi |U ) is defined by the conditional expectation of the test Yi given the variable U. The values of the ‘true score variable’ τi are the conditional expected values E(Yi|U= u) of Yi given the unit u. They are also called the ‘true scores’ of the unit u with respect to i. Hence, these true scores are the expected values of the intraindividual distributions of the Yi. The ‘measurement error variables’ εi are simply defined by the difference εi: -Yii. Table 1 summarizes the primitives and definitions of the basic concepts of CTT.

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

3. Properties Of True Score And Error Variables

Once the true score variables and error variables are defined a number of properties (see Table 2) can be derived, some of which are known as the ‘axioms of CTT.’ However, since the work done by Novick (1966) and Zimmerman (1975, 1976) it is well known that all these properties already follow from the definition of true score and error variables. They are not new and independent assumptions as has been originally proposed (e.g., Gulliksen 1950). All equations in Table 2 are no assumptions. They are inherent properties of true scores and errors. Hence, trying to test or falsify these properties empirically would be meaningless in just the same way, as it is meaningless to test whether or not a bachelor is really unmarried. The property of being unmarried is an inherent part or logical consequence of the concept of a bachelor.

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

Only one of the ‘axioms of CTT’ does not follow from the definition of true score and error variables: ‘uncorrelatedness of errors variables’ among each other. Hence, uncorrelatedness of errors has another epistemological status as the properties displayed in Table 2 (the other ‘axioms’). Uncorrelatedness of errors is certainly a desirable and useful property; but it might be wrong in specific empirical applications (e.g., Zimmerman and Williams 1977). In fact it is an assumption and it plays a crucial rule in defining models of CTT.

Equation 1 of Table 2 is a simple rearrangement of the definition of the error variable. Equation. 2 shows that the variance of a test score variable, too, has two additive components: the ‘variance of the true score variable’ and the ‘variance of the error variable.’ This second property follows from Eqn. 3 according to which a true score variable is uncorrelated with a measurement error variable, even if they pertain to different test score variables Yi and Yj. Equation 4 states that the expected value of an error variable is zero, whereas Eqn. 5 implies that the expected value of an error variable is zero within each individual observational unit u. Finally, according to Eqn. 6 the conditional expectation of an error variable is also zero for each mapping of U. This basically means that the expected value of an error variable is zero in each subpopulation of observational units.

3.1 Additional Concepts: Reliability, Unconditional And Conditional Error Variances

Although the true score and error variables defined above are the core concepts of CTT, in empirical applications, the true scores can only be estimated. What is also possible, is to estimate the ‘variances’ of the true score and error variables in a random sample (consisting of repeating many times the random experiment described earlier). The variance Var (εi) of the measurement error may be considered a gross parameter representing the degree of unreliability. A normed parameter of unreliability is Var (εi) Var (Yi), the proportion of the variance of Yi due to measurement error. Its counterpart is 1-Var (εi)/Var (Yi), i.e.,

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

the ‘reliability’ of Yi. This coefficient varies between zero and one. In fact, most theorems and most empirical research deal with this ‘coefficient of reliability.’ The reliability coefficient is a convenient information about the dependability of the measurement ‘in one single number.’

In early papers on CTT, reliability of a test has been defined by its correlation with itself (e.g., Thurstone 1931, p. 3). However, this definition is only metaphoric, because a variable always correlates perfectly with itself. What is meant is to define reliability by the correlation of ‘parallel tests’ (see below). The assumptions defining parallel tests in fact imply that the correlation between two test score variables is the reliability. Note that the definition of ‘reliability’ via Eqn. (1) does not rest on any assumption other than 0<Var (Yi)<∞.

‘Reliability’ is useful to compare different instruments to each other if they are applied in the same population. Used in this way, reliability in fact helps evaluating the quality of measurement instruments. However, it may not be useful under all circumstances to infer the dependability of measures of an individual unit. For the latter purpose one might rather look at the ‘conditional error variance’ Var (εi |U= u) given a specific observational unit u or at the ‘conditional error variances’ Var (εii=t ) given the subpopulation with true score τi = t.

4. Models of Classical Test Theory

The definitions of true score and error variables have to be supplemented by assumptions defining a model if the theoretical parameters such as the reliability are to be computed by empirically estimable parameters such as the means, variances, covariance, or correlation of the test score variables. Table 3 displays the most important of these assumptions and the most important models defined by combining some of these assumptions.

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

The assumption (a ) to (a ) specify in different ways the assumption that two tests Yi and Yj measure the same attribute. Such an assumption is crucial for inferring the degree of reliability from the discrepancy between two measurements of the same attribute of the same person. Perfect identity or ‘τ-equivalence’ of the two true score variables is assumed with (a ). With (a ) this assumption is relaxed: the two true score variables may differ by an additive constant. Two balances, for instance, will follow this assumption if one of them yields a weight that is always one pound larger than the weight indicated by the other balance, irrespective of the object to be weighed. According to Assumption (a ), the two tests measure the same attribute in the sense that their true score variables are linear functions of each other.

The other two assumptions deal with properties of the measurement errors. With (b) one assumes measurement errors pertaining to different test score variables to be uncorrelated. In (c) ‘equal error variances are assumed,’ i.e., these tests are assumed to measure equally well.

4.1 Parallel Tests

4.1.1 Definition. The most simple and convenient set of assumptions is the model of ‘parallel tests.’ Two tests Yi and Yj are defined to be parallel if they are τ-equivalent, if their error variables are uncorrelated, and if they have identical error variances. Note that Assumption (a ) implies that there is a uniquely defined latent variable being identical to each of the true score variables. Hence, one may drop the index i and denote this latent variable by η. The assumption of t-equivalence may equivalently be written Yi=η+εi, where εi : =Yi E(Yi|U ).

4.1.2 Identification. For parallel tests the theoretical parameters may be computed from the para- meters characterizing the distribution of at least two test score variables, i.e., the theoretical parameters are identified in this model if m ≥2. According to Table 4 the expected value of η is equal to the expected value of each of the tests, whereas the variance of η can be computed from the covariance of two different tests. The variance Var (εi) of the measurement error variables may be computed by the difference Var (Yi)-Co (Yi, Yj), i≠j. Finally, the reliability Rel(Yi) is equal to the correlation Corr(Yi, Yj) of two different test score variables.

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

4.1.3 Testability. The model of parallel tests implies several consequences that may be tested empirically. First, all parallel tests Yi have equal expectations E(Yi), equal variances Var (Yi), and equal covariances Co (Yi, Yj) in the total population. Second, parallel tests also have equal expectations within each subpopulation (see Table 4).

Note that these hypotheses may be tested separately and or simultaneously as a single multidimensional hypothesis in the framework of ‘simultaneous equation models’ via AMOS (Arbuckle 1997), EQS (Bentler 1995), LISREL 8 (Joereskog and Soerbom 1998), MPLUS (Muthen and Muthen 1998), MX (Neale 1997), RAMONA (Browne and Mels 1998), SEPATH (Steiger 1995), and others. Such a simultaneous test may even include the hypotheses about the parameters in several subpopulations (see Table 4). What is not implied by the assumptions of parallel tests is the equality of the variances and the co- variances of the test score variables in subpopulations.

For parallel tests Yn+…+Ym as defined in Table 4, the reliability of the sum score S : =Y1+…+ Ym may be computed by the ‘Spearman-Brown formula for lengthened tests:’

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

Using this formula the reliability of an aggregated measurement consisting of the sum (or average) of m parallel measurements of the same unit can be computed. For m =2, each with Rel(Yi)= 0.80, for instance

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

The Spearman–Brown formula may also be used to answer the opposite question. Suppose there is a test being the sum of m parallel tests and this test has reliability Rel(S). What would be the reliability Rel(Yi) of the m parallel tests? For example, if m =2, what would be the reliability of a test half?

4.2 Essentially τ-Equivalent Tests

4.2.1 Definition. The model of essentially τ-equivalent tests is less restrictive than the model of parallel tests. Two tests Yi and Yj are defined to be ‘essentially τ-equivalent’ if their true score variables differ only by an additive constant (Assumption a2 in Table 3) and if their error variables are uncorrelated (Assumption b in Table 3). Assumption (a2) implies that there is a latent variable η that is a translation of each of the true score variables, i.e.,

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

Also note that the latent variable η is uniquely defined up to a translation. Hence, it is necessary to fix the scale of the latent variable η. This can be done by fixing one of the coefficients λi (e.g., λ1= 0) or by fixing the expected value of η [e.g., E(η)=0].

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

Table 5 summarizes the assumptions defining the model and the consequences for identification and testability. In this model, the reliability cannot be identified any more by the correlation between two tests. Instead the reliability is identified by

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

Furthermore, the expected values of different tests are not identical any more within each subpopulation. Instead, the differences between the expected values E(s)(Yi) E(s)(Yj) of two essentially τ-equivalent tests Yi and Yj are the same in each and every subpopulation. All other properties are the same as in the model of parallel tests. Again, all these hypotheses may be tested via structural equation modeling.

For essentially τ-equivalent tests Y1, …, Ym, the reliability of the sum score S: =Y1+…+Ym may be computed by the ‘Cronbach’s coefficient α:’

This coefficient is a lower bound for the reliability of S if only uncorrelated errors are assumed.

4.3 τ-Congeneric Tests

4.3.1 Definition. The model of τ-congeneric tests is defined by the Assumptions (a3) and (b) in Table 3. Hence, two tests Yi and Yj are called τ-congeneric if their true score variables are positive linear functions of each other and if their error variables are uncorrelated. Assumption a3 implies that there is a latent variable η such that each true score variable is a positive linear function of it, i.e.,

or equivalently:

where εi:=Yi-E(Yi |U).

The latent variable η is uniquely defined up to positive linear functions. Hence, in this model, too, it is necessary to fix the scale of η. This can be done by fixing a pair of the coefficients (e.g., λi0= 0 and λi1=1) or by fixing the expected value and the variance of η [e.g., E(η) = 0 and Var (η) = 1].

Table 6 summarizes the assumptions defining the model and the consequences for identification and testability assuming E(η) =0 and Var (η) =1. Other ways of fixing the scale of η would imply different formula. As can be seen from the formula in Table 6, the model of τ-congeneric variables and all its parameters are identified if there are at least three different tests for which Assumptions a and b hold. The covariance structure in the total population implied by the model may be tested empirically if there are at least four test score variables. Only in this case the model has fewer theoretical parameters determining the covariance matrix of the test score variables than there elements in this covariance matrix. The implications for the mean structure are testable already for three test score variables provided the means of the test score variables are available in at least four subpopulations.

Classical Test Theory Research Paper

4.4 Other Models Of CTT

The models treated previously are not the only ones that can be used to determine the theoretical parameters of CTT such as reliability, true score variance, and error variance. In fact, the models dealt with are limited to unidimensional models. However, true score variables may also be decomposed into several latent variables. ‘Confirmatory factor analysis’ provides a powerful methodology to construct, estimate, and test models with multidimensional decompositions of true score variables. Note, however, that not each factor model is based on CTT. For instance, there are one-factor models that are not models of τ-congeneric variables in terms of CTT. A model with one common factor and several specific but uncorrelated factors is a counter example. The common factor is not necessarily a linear function of the true score variables and the specific factors are not necessarily the measurement error variables as defined in CTT.

4.5 Some Practical Issues

Once a measurement or test score has been obtained for a specific individual, one might want to know how dependable that individual measurement is. If the reliability of the measurement is known and if one assumes a normal distribution of the measurement errors which is homogeneous for all individuals, the 95 percent-confidence interval for the true score of that individual with respect to the measurement Yi can be computed by:

Another result deals with the correlation between two true score variables. If the reliabilities for two test score variables Y1 and Y2 are known, and assuming uncorrelated measurement errors, one may compute

This eqn. is known as the ‘correction for attenuation.’

Another important issue deals with the ‘reliability of a difference variable,’ for example, a difference between a pretest Y and a posttest Y . Assuming equal true score and error variances between pre and posttest implies identical reliabilities, i.e., Rel(Y1)=Rel(Y2)= Rel(Y ). If additionally uncorrelated measurement errors are assumed, the reliability Rel(Y1-Y2) := Var (E(Y1-Y2| U)/Var (Y1– Y2) of the difference Y1-Y2 may be computed by:

According to this formula, the reliability of a difference between pre- and posttest is always smaller than the reliability of the pre- and posttest, provided the assumptions mentioned above hold. In the extreme case in which there is no differential change, i.e., τ21+ constant, the reliability coefficient Rel(Y1-Y2) will be zero. Obviously, this does not mean that the change is not dependable. It only means that there is no variance in the change, since each individual changes by the same amount. This phenomenon has lead to much confusion about the usefulness of measuring change (e.g., Cronbach and Furby 1970, Harris 1963, Rogosa 1995). Most of these problems are now solved by structural equation modeling (allowing the include latent change variables such as in growth curve models (e.g., McArdle and Epstein 1987, Willet and Sayer 1996) or, more directly, in true change models (Steyer, Eid, and Schwenkmezger 1997, Steyer, Partchev, and Shanahan, 2000). Models of this kind are no longer hampered by reliability problems and allow the explanation of inter-individual differences in intraindividual change.

5. Discussion

It should be noted that CTT refers to the population level, i.e., to the random experiment of sampling a single observational unit and assessing some of its behavior (see Table 1). CTT does not refer to sampling models that consist of repeating this random experiment many times. Hence, no questions of statistical estimation and hypothesis testing are dealt with. Of course, the population models of CTT have to be supplemented by sampling models when it comes to applying statistical analyses, e.g., via structural equation modeling.

Aside from this more technical aspect, what are the limitations of CTT? First, CTT and its models are not really adequate for modeling answers to individual items in a questionnaire. This purpose is more adequately met by models of item response theory (IRT) which specify how the probability of answering in a specific category of an item depends on the attribute to be measured, i.e., on the value of a latent variable.

A second limitation of CTT is the exclusive focus on measurement errors. ‘Generalizability theory’ presented by Cronbach et al. (1972) (see also Shavelson and Webb 1991) generalized CTT to include other factors determining test scores.

Inspired by Generalizability Theory Tack (1980), Steyer et al. (1989) presented a generalization of CTT, called ‘Latent State-Trait Theory,’ which explicitly takes into account the situation factor, introduced formal definitions of states and traits, and presented models allowing to disentangle person, as well as situation and or interaction effects and from measurement error. More recent presentations are Steyer et al. (1992) as well as Steyer, et al. (1999). Eid (1995, 1996) extended this approach to the normal ogive model for analyses on the item level.

The parameters of CTT are often said to be ‘population dependent,’ i.e., meaningful only with respect to a given population. This is true for the variance of the true score variable and the reliability coefficient. The reliability (coefficient) of an intelligence test is different in the population of students than the general population. This is a simple consequence of the restriction of the (true score) variance of intelligence in the population of the students. However, such a restriction neither exists for the true score estimates neither of individual persons nor for the item parameters λi of the model of ‘essentially τ-equivalent tests,’ for instance. Proponents of IRT models have often forwarded the population dependence critique. They contrast it with the ‘population independence’ of the person and the item parameters of IRT models. However, ‘population independence’ also holds for the person and the item parameters of the model of essentially τ-equivalent tests, for instance.

In applications of CTT it is often assumed that the error variances are the same for each individual, irrespective of the true score of that individual. This assumption may indeed be wrong in many applications. In IRT models no such assumption is made. However, it is possible to assume different error variances for different (categories of ) persons in CTT models as well. In this case, the unconditional error variance and the reliability coefficient are not the best available information for inferring the dependability of individual true score estimates. In this case one should seek to obtain estimates of conditional measurement error variances for specific classes of persons. It is to be expected that persons with high true scores have a higher measurement error variance than those with medium true scores and that those with low true scores have a higher error variance again. (This would be due to ‘floor and ceiling effects.’) Other patterns of the error variance depending on the size of the true score may occur as well.

Such phenomena do not mean that ‘true scores’ and ‘error scores’ would be correlated; only the ‘error variances’ would depend on the true scores. None of the properties listed in Table 2 would be violated. As mentioned before, the properties listed in Table 2 cannot be wrong in empirical applications. What could be wrong, however, is the true score interpretation of the latent variable in a concrete structural equation model. Misinterpretations of this sort can be most effectively prevented by empirical tests of the hypotheses listed in the testability sections of Tables 4 to 6.

The most challenging critique of many applications of CTT is that they are based on rather arbitrarily defined test score variables. If these test score variables are not well chosen any model based on them is also not well founded. Are there really good reasons to base models on sum scores across items in questionnaires? Why take the sum of the items as test score variables Yi and not another way of aggregation such as a weighted sum, or a product, or a sum of logarithms? And why aggregate and not look at the items themselves?

There is no doubt that IRT models are more informative than CTT models if samples are big enough to allow their application, if the items obey the laws defining the models, and if detailed information about the items (and even about the categories of ‘polytomous items,’ such as in ‘ratings scales’) is sought. In most applications, the decision how to define the test score variables Yi on which models of CTT are built is arbitrary, to some degree. It should be noted, however, that arbitrariness in the choice of the test score variables cannot be avoided altogether. Even if models are based on the item level, such as in IRT models, one may ask ‘Why these items and not other ones?’ Whether or not a good choice has been made will only prove in model tests and in validation studies. This is true for models of CTT as well as for models of alternative theories of psychometric tests.

Bibliography:

  1. Arbuckle J L 1997 Amos User’s Guide: Version 3.6. SPSS, Chicago, IL
  2. Bentler P M 1995 EQS. Structural Equation Program Manual. Multivariate Software, Encino
  3. Browne M W, Mels G 1998 Path analysis (RAMONA). In: SYSTAT 8.0 -Statistics. SPSS, Inc., Chicago
  4. Cronbach L J, Furby L 1970 How should we measure ‘change’—or should we? Psychological Bulletin 74: 68–80
  5. Cronbach L J, Gleser G C, Nanda H, Rajaratnam N 1972 The Dependability of Behavioral Measurements: Theory of Generalizability of Scores and Profiles. Wiley, New York
  6. du Toit M, du Toit S 2001 Interactive LISREL: User’s Guide. Scientific Software International, Chicago
  7. Eid M 1995 Modelle der Messung on Personen in Situationen [Models of measuring persons in situations]. Psychologie Verlags Union, Weinheim, Germany
  8. Eid M 1996 Longitudinal confirmatory factor analysis for polytomous item responses: model definition and model selection on the basis of stochastic measurement theory [online]. http://www.ppm.ipn.uni-kiel.de/mpr/issue1/art4 eid.pdf: 1999-7-5
  9. Fischer G H, Molenaar I W 1995 Rasch Models. Springer, New York
  10. Gulliksen H 1950 Theory of Mental Tests. Wiley, New York
  11. Joreskog K G, Sorbom D 1998 LISREL 8. Users Reference Guide. Scientific Software, Chicago
  12. Lord F M, Novick M R 1968 Statistical Theories of Mental Test Scores. Addison Wesley, Reading, MA
  13. Muthen L, Muthen B 1998 Mplus User’s Guide. Muthen & Muthen, Los Angeles
  14. Neale M C 1997 MX: Statistical Modeling, 4th edn. Department of Psychiatry, Richmond, VA
  15. Novick M R 1966 The axioms and principal results of classical test theory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 3: 1–18
  16. Rogosa D 1995 Myths and methods: ‘Myths about longitudinal research’ plus supplemental questions. In: Gottman J M (ed.) The Analysis of Change. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ
  17. Shavelson R J, Webb N M 1991 Generalizability Theory. A Primer. Sage, Newbury Park
  18. Steiger J H 1995 Structural equation modeling. In: STATISTICA 5—Statistics II. StatSoft Inc., Tulsa, OK
  19. Steyer R, Ferring D, Schmitt M J 1992 States and traits in psychological assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 8: 79–98
  20. Steyer R, Majcen A-M, Schwenkmezger P, Buchner A 1989 A latent state-trait anxiety model and its application to determine consistency and specificity coeffi Anxiety Research 1: 281–99
  21. Steyer R, Partchev I, Shanahan M 2000 Modeling true intraindividual change in structural equation models: the case of poverty and children’s psychosocial adjustment. In: Little T D, Schnabel K U, Baumert J (eds.) Modeling Longitudinal and Multiple-Group Data: Practical Issues, Applied Approaches, and Specific Examples. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 109–26
  22. Steyer R, Schmitt M, Eid M 1999 Latent state-trait theory and research in personality and individual diff European Journal of Personality
  23. Tack W H 1980 Zur Theorie psychometrischer Verfahren: Formalisierung der Erfassung von Situationsabhangigkeit und Veranderung [On the theory of psychometric procedures: formalizing the assessment of situational dependency and change]. Zeitschrift fur Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie 1: 87–106
  24. Thurstone L L 1931 The Reliability and Validity of Tests. Edwards Brothers, Ann Arbor
  25. Zimmerman D W 1975 Probability spaces, hilbert spaces, and the axioms of test theory. Psychometrika 40: 395–412
  26. Zimmerman D W 1976 Test theory with minimal assumptions. Educational and Psychological Measurement 36: 85–96
  27. Zimmerman D W, Williams R H 1977 The theory of test validity and correlated errors of measurement. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 16: 135–52
Classification In The Social Sciences Research Paper
Internal Structure of Cities Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!