Stress and Cardiac Response Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

View sample Stress and Cardiac Response Research Paper. Browse other  research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. If you need a religion research paper written according to all the academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help. This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our custom writing services for professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable rates.

The origin of the concept of stress in health-related disciplines is closely linked to the idea that organisms react physiologically to the presence of danger or threat. This reactivity has a protective function since it provides the logistic and instrumental basis for adaptive behaviors such as fight or flight. However, if maintained for long periods, this protective or defensive reactivity may become a health risk, compromising the normal functioning of the organs involved. For many scientists the excessive physiological reactivity associated with stress is the main mechanism linking stress to illness.

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


1. Cardiac Response

The heart is a vital organ that reacts rapidly to physical and psychological demands. This reactivity is controlled by the central nervous system through neural and humoral pathways.

1.1 Neural Pathways

The neural pathway to the heart involves the two branches of the autonomic nervous system: the sympathetic and the parasympathetic. When the sympathetic nerves are activated, the heart rate accelerates and the heart contraction becomes stronger, resulting in a greater blood volume discharge and greater blood pressure in the vessels. When the parasympathetic nerves are activated, the heart rate decelerates, reducing the blood volume discharge and the blood pressure. The two branches can act reciprocally (one activated and the other inhibited), working in the same direction (increasing or decreasing the heart activity), or nonreciprocally (both activated or inhibited simultaneously), competing against each other for the final cardiac outcome.




1.2 Humoral Pathways

The humoral pathway involves the neural activation of the endocrine system, resulting in the secretion by the adrenal glands of various hormones into the bloodstream. One of these hormones, adrenaline, when it reaches the heart, has the same effect as the sympathetic nerves: it increases the heart rate and the stroke volume. Other adrenal hormones, such as cortisol, facilitate the transfer of lipids and glucose into the bloodstream and also have an important indirect effect on the heart and on the arteries that supply blood to the heart muscle: the coronaries. The humoral effects on the heart are slower than the neural effects but they last longer, contributing to maintaining cardiac reactivity during prolonged periods.

1.3 Cardiac–Respiratory Interactions

The cardiovascular and the respiratory systems are both responsible for maintaining the continuous exchange of gases, oxygen and carbon dioxide, between the atmosphere and the tissues. The cardiovascular system reacts to changes in the concentration of these gases in the blood. Therefore, changes in the respiratory pattern which induce changes in gas concentration produce changes in the blood flow and cardiac activity in order to restore the appropriate gas concentration. Two examples of cardiac reactivity due to changes in respiration are the hyper ventilation and respiratory sinus arrhythmia phenomena.

1.3.1 Hyper Ventilation. Hyperventilation is caused by rapid respiratory patterns provoking reduced carbon dioxide concentration in the blood. The cardiovascular response is a reduced blood flow to the brain and heart arteries, and an increase in cardiac activity due to parasympathetic inhibition.

1.3.2 Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia. In contrast, respiratory sinus arrhythmia is caused by slow and relaxed respiratory patterns. The consequence is a rhythmic deceleration of the heart rate, due to parasympathetic activation, coinciding with each expiratory cycle.

2. Stress And Cardiac Defense

Cardiac defense can be defined as the cardiac response to perceived danger or threat. The concept of defense has a long tradition in physiology and psychology and it is in this context that the concept of stress was first introduced.

2.1 Historical Antecedents

At the beginning of the twentieth century, Ivan Pavlov and other Russian reflexologists used the term defense reflex to refer to protective physiological responses elicited by noxious stimulation, such as hand withdrawal from an electric shock, eyeblink to a puff of air, or vomit to bad food. A few years later, Walter Cannon used the term defense to refer to the ‘fight or flight’ response, a sympathetically mediated cardiovascular response to emergency situations aimed at providing energy supply to the body to facilitate adaptive behaviors such as attack (fight) or escape (flight). By the middle of the century and following Cannon’s ideas, Hans Selye had introduced the concept of stress and used the term ‘alarm’ response to refer to the first stage of the physiological response to stressful situations.

2.2 Classical Approaches To Cardiac Defense

There have been two major approaches to the study of cardiac defense: (a) the cognitive or sensory processing approach built on Pavlov’s distinction between orienting and defense reflexes, and (b) the motivational or emotional approach built on Cannon’s ideas on the fight or flight response.

2.2.1 The Cognitive Approach. The cognitive approach assumes that cardiac changes to environmental stimuli reflect attentional and perceptual mechanisms aimed at facilitating or inhibiting the processing of the stimuli (Sokolov 1963, Graham 1979). The orienting response (a deceleration of the heart rate to moderate or novel stimulation) facilitates attention and perception of the stimuli, whereas the defense response (an acceleration of the heart rate to intense or aversive stimulation) reduces attention and perception as a form of protection against the stimuli. To study cardiac defense, this approach has developed tasks that present discrete sensory stimuli, such as noise, light, pictures, or words, manipulating their physical features—intensity, duration, and rise time—or their affective content—pleasant vs. unpleasant pictures or words. According to this approach, cardiac defense is characterized by a heart rate accelerative response, peaking between 3 and 6 s after stimulus onset, which habituates slowly with stimulus repetition.

2.2.2 The Motivational Approach. The motivational approach assumes that cardiac changes in response to environmental demands reflect metabolic mechanisms aimed at providing the necessary energy to the body to support behavioral adjustments (Obrist 1981). If the appropriate behavior is to be quiet and relaxed, then the cardiac response will be a heart-rate deceleration. If the appropriate behavior is to be active, either physically or psychologically, then the cardiac response will be a heart-rate acceleration.

To study cardiac defense, this approach has developed tasks that involve emotionally or cognitively challenging situations maintained throughout several minutes, such as keeping the hand in cold water, pressing a key as fast as possible to avoid shocks, or doing complex arithmetic tasks. The term ‘mental stress’ was introduced in this context to refer to tasks that require ‘mental effort’ instead of ‘physical effort.’ Under such conditions cardiac defense is characterized by a heart-rate acceleration maintained during the task duration, accompanied by reduced cardiac variability linked to hyperventilation and loss of respiratory sinus arrythmia.

3. A New Approach To Cardiac Defense

The cognitive and motivational approaches to cardiac defense have been difficult to reconcile in the past. However, new data have been accumulated on defense responses, both in animals and humans, and on the neurophysiological brain circuits controlling such responses, which not only facilitate integration of the classic approaches but also have potential implications for the concept of stress and its relation to cardiovascular illness.

3.1 Natural Defense

In natural settings, such as the imminence of a predator, the defense reaction is not a unique response that could be conceptualized as cognitive vs. motivational, or vice versa. Rather, the defense reaction follows a dynamic sequential process with initial phases in which attentional factors predominate, aimed at the detection and analysis of the potential danger, and if the process continues, later phases in which behavioral factors predominate, aimed at active defense such as fight or flight. This sequential process can be divided into three stages: pre-encounter, postencounter, and circa strike. Depending on the spatial and temporal proximity of the predator, different components of the defense reaction may take place successively.

3.2 Protective Reflexes

Motor startle and cardiac defense are two protective reflexes that have been extensively studied in recent years in relation to both cognitive and emotional modulatory factors. Startle is a whole-body motor flexor response, including an eyeblink that is elicited by abrupt stimulation such as a brief, intense noise or light. Startle is augmented when the organism is in an aversive emotional state and inhibited when the organism is in a positive emotional state. Similarly, startle is potentiated in the first stages of the natural defense reaction when attentional factors predominate, and inhibited during the later phases when behavioral factors predominate (Lang 1995).

On the other hand, studies on the cardiac components of the defense response to unexpected noises or shocks have questioned the classical models on response characteristics and their functional significance (Vila 1995, Vila et al. 1997). The cardiac response to an unexpected noise is not only a heartrate acceleration. The response is a complex pattern of heart-rate changes, observed throughout the 80 s after stimulus onset, with two large accelerative and two smaller decelerative components in alternating order (see Fig. 1). The evocation of the response has both parasympathetic (first acceleration and deceleration) and sympathetic (second acceleration and deceleration) influences. As regards its psychological significance, contrary to the classical cognitive model, cardiac defense is accompanied by an increase in sensory processing (sensory intake). On the other hand, emotional factors are also involved, producing similar modulatory effects as in motor startle. For example, phobic subjects presented with aversive noise while viewing pictures of their phobic object show a clear augmentation of the two accelerative components of the response.

Stress and Cardiac Response Research Paper Figure 1

3.3 The Brain’s Defense System

The above data support a description and interpretation of cardiac defense as a sequence of heart-rate changes with both accelerative and decelerative components, with both parasympathetic and sympathetic influences, and with both cognitive (attentional) and emotional (energetic) significance. The structural foundation of this response is in the brain’s aversive motive system. What is known about this system comes from animal research, particularly from studies on defense reactions and fear conditioning (see LeDoux 1996 and Lang et al. 1997 for a comprehensive review). Neuroscientists have traced the chain of neural activation in the brain, starting from the sensory input, proceeding through the subcortical and cortical connecting structures, and finishing in the autonomic and motor effectors. A critical structure in this circuit is the amygdala, a subcortical center located within the temporal lobes that receives inputs from the sensory thalamus and various cortical structures. Downstream from the amygdala, the defense circuit branches into different structures, controlling separate forms of defense. This hierarchical organization, together with the learned plasticity in the anatomy and functioning of the brain’s structures, provides considerably variety in the pattern of defensive output. Depending on the situational context and on the weight of the local pathways within the circuit, the aversively motivated responses can be escape or avoidance, aggressive behavior, hormonal and autonomic responses, or the augmentation of motor startle.

4. Conclusions: Implications For Stress And Cardiovascular Disease

The classical models of defense did not emphasize the dynamic character of the defense response—obvious in natural settings—and the simultaneous involvement of different psychological processes—cognitive and motivational. The new model matches better the prevalent model of stress in health-related disciplines: the cognitive-transactional model. The continuous dynamic interaction between the situation and the person, leading to activation or deactivation of the stress response, is the main feature of the transactional model. In addition, the new approach to defense can help to understand the specific nature of stress, differentiating it from other related concepts such as anxiety or arousal. Based on the new defense model, stress can be defined as the sustained activation of the brain’s defense motivational system. A state of maintained activation of this system implies a variety of specific defense responses being continuously elicited, including neural and humoral cardiovascular responses that correlate with poor cardiovascular health: maintained increases in heart rate, stroke volume and blood pressure, and maintained decreases in respiratory sinus arrhythmia and cardiac variability. These sustained responses are instrumental for the development of cardiovascular diseases such as essential hypertension and coronary heart disease.

Bibliography:

  1. Graham F K 1979 Distinguishing among orienting, defense and startle reflexes. In: Kimmel H D, van Olst E H, Orlebeke F J (eds.) The Orienting Reflex in Humans. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 137–67
  2. Lang P J 1995 The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist 50: 372–85
  3. Lang P J, Bradley M M, Cuthbert B N 1997 Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. In: Lang P J, Simons R F, Balaban M T (eds.) Attention and Orienting. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 97–135
  4. LeDoux J 1996 The Emotional Brain. Pergamon, New York
  5. Obrist P A 1981 Cardio vascular Psychophysiology: A Perspective. Simon & Schuster, New York
  6. Sokolov E N 1963 Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. Pergamon, Oxford, UK
  7. Vila J 1995 Cardiac psychophysiology and health. In: RodrıguezMarın J (ed.) Health Psychology and Quality of Life Research. University of Alicante Press, Alicante, Spain, pp. 520–40
  8. Vila J, Perez M N, Fernandez M D, Pegalajar J, Sanchez M 1997 Attentional modulation of the cardiac defense response in humans. Psychophysiology 34: 482–7
Stress and Coping Theories Research Paper
Street Children and Psychology Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!