Cognitive Psychology Of Serial Order Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

View sample Cognitive Psychology Of Serial Order Research Paper. Browse other research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. iResearchNet offers academic assignment help for students all over the world: writing from scratch, editing, proofreading, problem solving, from essays to dissertations, from humanities to STEM. We offer full confidentiality, safe payment, originality, and money-back guarantee. Secure your academic success with our risk-free services.

Stimuli come and go. The serial order of their appearance is determined by rules in the external world as well as by the actions of the organism experiencing the stimuli. Consequently, successions of stimuli are almost never random but rather comprise some form of structure. Structure allows prediction, and predicting forthcoming events is advantageous because organisms can prepare for what will happen. Thus, sensitivity to serial order can be considered as being one of the basic mechanisms by which humans (and organisms in general) adapt behavior to constraints in the succession of events. That is why the examination of serial learning has a prolonged research history (e.g., Ebbinghaus 1902, Lashley 1951, Miller 1956).

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


Serial order refers to deviations from randomness in the succession of events. Consider, for instance, the three colored lights green (g), red (r), and yellow (y) as distinct events. The series g–g–r–g–y–g–r–y–y–g–r–… is random, as each of the lights was selected with equal probability in each position. In contrast, the series g–y–r–y–g–y–r–y–g … presents the highly regular sequence we experience in watching ordinary traffic lights. Knowing the serial order of traffic lights facilitates responding: if we see that the light switches from green to yellow we can expect that it will next switch to red so that we can prepare to stop before we are really forced to do so. Almost all event sequences entail some regularity: clouds are frequently followed by rain, words follow each other according to grammatical rules, tone sequences constitute melodies, and baking a cake requires an ordered sequence of actions, etc. In this research paper, first two types of serial order are distinguished, then several effects of serial order on human cognition are sketched, and finally, the main theoretical approaches to account for serial order effects are mentioned.

1. Statistical And Relational Constraints In Serial Order

Statistical constraints refer to the conditional probabilities of events in dependence on preceding ones. Traffic lights are a good example: after ‘red’ and after ‘green,’ ‘yellow’ is certain to follow, whereas after ‘yellow’ either ‘red’ or ‘green’ follows. If one takes two preceding lights into account, however, the light following ‘green–yellow’ and ‘red–yellow’ is also safely predictable. An example of more complex statistical constraints is provided by sentences: the next word in a sentence can mostly be predicted with a probability higher than chance but almost never with certainty, regardless of how many preceding words are taken into account. Relational constraints refer to regularities in the relations between successive events instead of conditional probabilities. Consider, for instance, an arithmetic series such as 3–8–13–18–23–28. The regularity of the series is based on that between every pair of adjacent numbers the same relation holds. An example of a somewhat less regular relational structure is a digit sequence such as 23461938, which contains only two equally related pairs of numbers, namely 23–46 and 19–38.




2. Serial Order Effects In Human Cognition

2.1 Serial Order Effects In Prediction

In order to examine the adaptation of predictions to serial order, experiments have been performed in which items are successively presented and participants are asked to predict each forthcoming item. Typically, prediction accuracy improves corresponding to the statistical and/or relational constraints in the succession of items. Moreover, the data indicate that constraints in immediate successions are effective before constraints in the succession of items further apart from each other are taken into account. Consider, for instance, a well ordered sequence such as 1–2–1–2–2–3–2–3–6–5–6–5–5–4–5–4. Experimental data show that participants first learn to predict the respective second items of those pairs whose elements are likewise related, (12), (23), (65), and (54). They continue to learn the repetitions of pairs (1212), (2323), (6565), and (5454). Finally, participants manage to predict the order of the quadruples (Restle and Brown 1970). In other words, serial learning starts by connecting adjacent items into subunits or chunks corresponding to the given relational (as in the present case) or statistical constraints. Following this, constraints in the succession of these elementary chunks results in the formation of higher order chunks, i.e., connections between elementary chunks, which again are subject to chunking, etc. Thus, consecutive chunking may lead to a representation of the whole sequence through a hierarchy of chunks.

2.2 Serial Order Effects In Remembering

Ordered sequences of items are generally remembered better than unordered sequences. For instance, sequences of words are remembered better if they follow the statistical constraints of word order in sentences (Miller and Selfridge 1950), a list of words is remembered better if words belonging to the same category (e.g., names of fishes, flowers, birds, or trees) are presented adjacently (Underwood et al. 1974), and a series of letters will be remembered better if it consists of familiar abbreviations as in FBICIAPHDVIP. In all these cases subsequences of the items to be remembered correspond to familiar knowledge units like word phrases, categories, or abbreviations which are used to reduce memory load. In the latter case, for instance, participants have to keep in mind the four abbreviations FBI, CIA, PHD, and VIP instead of 12 letters. Moreover, the order of the storage units or chunks may provide a useful retrieval plan which additionally helps remembering. In the categorically ordered word list mentioned above, for instance, memory will be further improved if also the categories are presented in a systematic order such as the animals (fishes and birds) first and then the plants (flowers and trees).

However, new and unfamiliar serial constraints also improve memory. Consider, for instance a so-called ‘finite state grammar,’ which restricts the way in which the letters T, P, V, X, and S are allowed to follow each other in letter strings. For instance, all grammatical strings may start either with the letter T or P, they may finish either with V or S, only the letters X and V may be allowed to be repeated, after letter T only the letters S, X, and V may be allowed to follow, etc. In this way the ‘grammar’ constitutes a statistical structure which all grammatical strings such as TXS, TSXXTVV, PTVPS, or PVPXVPS have in common. If such letter strings are to be memorized, grammatical strings are remembered increasingly better than random strings and after some training participants are even able to distinguish between grammatical and agrammatical strings fairly well (Reber 1967). The findings clearly indicate that the completely new and arbitrary statistical constraints have been learned and are used to put order into the letter strings to be remembered.

2.3 Serial Order Effects In Motor Performance

In motor performance, similar effects as in predicting or remembering stimulus sequences have been reported: well-ordered sequences of actions are executed faster and more correctly than unordered sequences and, even more informative, the timing of sequence execution mirrors its serial order (Rosenbaum et al. 1983). Consider, for instance, the times for pressing a series of eight distinct keys with the index fingers (I, i) and the middle fingers (M, m) of the right and left hand, respectively. The order of the keys to be pressed shall be such that a first pair of keystrokes (e.g., I–m) is to be repeated (I–m–I–m) and then a second pair is to be repeated (e.g., M–i–M–i). In performing such a regular keystroke sequence as fast as possible, execution times are highest at the beginning of the quadruples ImIm and MiMi, they are somewhat reduced at the beginning of each repeated pair ImIm and MiMi, and they are lowest for the respective second element in each pair ImIm and MiMi. The correspondence of this time pattern with the serial order of the keystrokes suggests that the time to execute a motor act in a sequence of acts is determined by hierarchically organized chunks rather in the same way as the prediction of items is: at the beginning of each new chunk execution time increases as prediction accuracy decreases and for items within chunks execution is fastest and prediction is best.

2.4 Serial Order Effects In Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Tasks

In SRT tasks, participants are required to respond to successively presented stimuli by distinct responses whereby each response triggers the presentation of the next stimulus, which in turn requires the next response, etc. Numerous studies have shown that nearly any kind of serial order in the succession of stimulus– response pairs results in a decrease in response times (Hoffmann and Koch 1998). Serial-order effects in SRT tasks deserve special attention because besides constraints in the succession of stimuli and of responses, also constraints in the succession of the responses and the following stimuli, i.e., in response–effect relations, may contribute to serial learning. Although not much research has been done yet in this respect, the available evidence clearly indicates that consistent response–effect relations do indeed contribute to the acquisition of serial order (Ziessler 1998). This is most noteworthy because consistent response–effect relations are an essential part of any ordinary behavioral sequence.

3. Intentional And Incidental Acquisition Of Serial Order

Some of the mentioned cognitive effects surely result from the explicit detection and use of serial order. For instance, if one tries to predict the continuation of a stimulus sequence on the basis of regularities in the preceding series, or if one tries to reduce memory load by parsing a string of letters into abbreviations, the given serial order is explicitly used to improve performance. However, there are other cases in which it does not seem to be necessary that serial order is detected in order to become effective (Stadler and Frensch 1998). For instance, memory for letter strings generated by a ‘finite state grammar’ improves without the grammatical constraints being reportable. Likewise, in SRT tasks participants respond increasingly faster to statistically ordered stimulus sequences without being aware of the statistical constraints, and consistent response–effect relations have also been shown to decrease response times without being detected. Obviously, serial order can become effective in two ways, once via a strategic use of detected constraints and second via more elementary mechanisms by which serial constraints seemingly inevitably affect performance, irrespective of whether they are detected or not.

4. Theoretical Accounts

The mechanisms underlying the acquisition and use of serial order are not yet well understood. However, it is generally acknowledged that serial learning is accomplished by processes that lead to the formation of chunks, i.e., to an integration of successive events into coherent subsequences (Miller 1956). The theoretical accounts differ with respect to the processes which are assumed to mediate the formation of chunks as a result of experienced serial constraints. On the one hand, explicit strategies for searching for serial order have been analyzed (Simon and Kotovski 1963). From this perspective, the formation of chunks results from the storage and comparison of subsequences explicitly in the search for identical structures which can be generalized to the whole sequence. On the other hand, it has been acknowledged that constraints in the succession of events also become incidentally effective. For instance, an implicit learning mechanism has been assumed that incidentally associates current preceding and forthcoming events if the former allow the prediction of the latter. Corresponding simulations in so-called recurrent networks have proven that adaptations to grammatical constraints may indeed result from such simple associative learning (Elman 1990, Cleeremans 1993). Other accounts have proposed that events are automatically integrated into a chunk if they are frequently and consecutively experienced in the same succession (Servan-Schreiber and Anderson 1990). Finally, the formation of response–effect associations has also been acknowledged as an autonomous process which may contribute to the formation of chunks in the sense of a series of effects which can be reliably predicted as the outcome of a certain sequence of actions (Hoffmann 1993). Thus, one can conclude that the acquisition and representation of serial order certainly is not a result of only a single process but rather the outcome of different mechanisms which all aim at improving the predictability of forthcoming events.

Bibliography:

  1. Cleeremans A 1993 Mechanisms of Implicit Learning. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
  2. Ebbinghaus H 1902 Grundzuge der Psychologie. Veit, Leipzig, Germany, Vol. 1
  3. Elman J L 1990 Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science 14: 179–211
  4. Hoffmann J 1993 Vorhersage und Erkenntnis: Die Funktion on Antizipationen in der menschlichen Verhaltenssteuerung und Wahrnehmung. Hogrefe, Gottingen, Germany
  5. Hoffmann J, Koch I 1998 Implicit learning of loosely defined structures. In: Stadler M A, Frensch P A (eds.) Handbook of Implicit Learning, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, pp. 161–99
  6. Lashley K S 1951 The problem of serial order in behavior. In: Jeffress L A (ed.) Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior. Wiley, New York, pp. 112–36
  7. Miller G A 1956 The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63: 81–97
  8. Miller G A, Selfridge J A 1950 Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material. American Journal of Psychology 63: 176–85
  9. Reber A S 1967 Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 6: 855–63
  10. Restle F, Brown E R 1970 Serial pattern learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 83: 120–5
  11. Rosenbaum D A, Kenny S B, Derr M A 1983 Hierarchical control of rapid movement sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9: 86–102
  12. Servan-Schreiber E, Anderson J R 1990 Learning artificial grammars with competitive chunking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16: 592–608
  13. Simon H A, Kotovski K 1963 Human acquisition of concepts for sequential patterns. Psychological Review 70: 534–46
  14. Stadler M A, Frensch P A (eds.) 1998 Handbook of Implicit Learning. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA
  15. Underwood B J, Shaughnessy J J, Zimmermann J 1974 The locus of the retention differences associated with the degree of hierarchical conceptual structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology 102: 850–62
  16. Ziessler M 1998 Response–effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24: 962–78
Schemas, Frames, And Scripts In Cognitive Psychology Research Paper
Production Systems In Cognitive Psychology Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!