Psychosomatic Medicine Research Paper

Academic Writing Service

Sample Psychosomatic Medicine Research Paper. Browse other  research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. If you need a religion research paper written according to all the academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help. This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our research paper writing service for professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable rates.

In its simplest sense, ‘psychosomatic medicine’ merely indicates that there is a (psyche) mind–body (soma) relationship relevant to human health. Drawing from this, many nonprofessionals believe that it is a medical specialty that concentrates on the psychological influences on disease, particularly certain ‘psychosomatic diseases.’ Actually, it is a health science that is concerned with the nature of the mind–body relationship, including its clinical applications.

Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services

Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code


1. Definition

More specifically, psychosomatic medicine is a field of health science based on the concept that the human organism is a psychosomatic unity, that biological and psychological processes are inextricably inter-related aspects of its function. This stands in contrast to the still predominant, traditional biomedical model (see Engel 1977) from the nineteenth century which holds that diseases represent cellular or organ pathology arising from intrinsic defects or external bio-physicochemical agents. Psychosomatic medicine conceptualizes diseases as occurring in persons, not their components, as a consequence of their transactions with an environment that is informational, social, and cultural, as well as bio-physicochemical. It notes that people are not simply biological organisms, but sentient ones with motives, thoughts, feelings, and relationships.

Mind–body unity should not be misunderstood to imply that mental and somatic phenomena cannot be separately studied. Such reductionism is a legitimate tactic that permits the study of scientific questions limited to that system. Each organ or part function merits study by techniques appropriate to it. To understand organismic function, however, requires a perspective that includes data from all body systems, including the psychological. Psychosomatic medicine provides no theories about the nature of the underlying processes. What it provides is its holistic perspective for interpreting them. It draws on the data, theory, and techniques of all other relevant fields, integrating these in unique ways informed by its conceptual base. Thus, contrary to common lay belief, psychosomatic medicine is not a medical specialty. Nor is it a scientific discipline. Its practitioners come from a range of disciplines and clinical specialties.




Some have accused psychosomatic medicine of overweighting the importance of psychosocial factors in disease, trivializing biology. This had legitimacy during the 1940s and 1950s when a prominent segment of the field fell into this trap (see Sect 2.3). If this seems still true, that is because its main effort is directed to clarifying the effects of psychosocial factors on the body. But that emphasis is necessitated by the neglect of this work by the remainder of biomedicine. Psychosomatic medicine is concerned with all mind–body relationships, giving equal importance to biology.

Doubts have been raised about the aptness of the term. To some, the bipartite nature of ‘psychosomatic’ undermines the intended unity. But it is one, unhyphenated word. Others question the absence of reference to the social, noting that many relevant factors are such. Engel’s (1977) alternative, ‘biopsychosocial model’ was meant to resolve that, as well as to circumvent the residual misinterpretation that biology is devalued. But if social is included, what about cultural? Indeed, the list could include economic and other factors. Moreover, these factors can impinge on the organism only via the psychological apparatus. ‘Psychosomatic medicine’ also has the virtue of historical continuity that includes a mass of research documentation.

2. History

2.1 Origins

From earliest times, people have been struck by instances in which physical symptoms followed noteworthy life events, and reports of these appear in ancient writings. With the beginnings of medical science, physicians added more such reports as well as describing illness outbreaks during wars and major upheavals. The idea that stressful life experiences caused illness gained wider currency. During the nineteenth century, philosophical conjectures about the holistic quality of human nature developed. This was the period when ‘psychosomatic’ came into use by physicians, although in a sense partly different from our own.

2.2 Early Twentieth Century

With the turn of the century, Cannon’s (1915) studies on the physiology of the emotions added key data revealing how these physiological responses were adapti e to the external threats that had triggered the emotions. Building on Claude Bernard’s earlier work, he indicated (1932) how his findings fit into the process by which organisms maintain their integrity by adapting to environmental change via internal responses to maintain internal equilibrium: ‘homeostasis.’

Clinical and epidemiological studies of better quality increased. By 1935, (Helen) Flanders Dunbar was able to cite some 2,251 references in her groundbreaking work, Emotions and Bodily Disease, which ushered in the modern, scientific era of psychosomatic medicine.

Further stimulus came from World War II. With astonishing foresight, the (US) National Research Council, anticipating casualties from war stress, convened a series of conferences among psychosomatic experts during the mid-1930s. This kindled additional research which culminated in 1939 with publication of the journal Psychosomatic Medicine, still the preeminent periodical in the field. Three years later, the American Psychosomatic Society was founded to foster scientific interchange and education. US military physicians observed how war trauma led to physical as well as mental disorders, and many returned with an interest in psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatic societies arose in other countries and, later, international meetings began.

Psychoanalysis provided further impetus. Freud and his followers were greatly interested in physical symptoms. Much of their theorizing involved a simplistic transfer of the psychoanalytic psychology of hysterical symptoms, without regard to physiology. Nevertheless, as psychoanalytic activity flourished, it stirred interest among intellectuals, adding to the popularity of psychosomatic medicine. With this came the naive belief that many medical disorders were primarily psychogenic.

2.3 Specificity Theories

Paralleling major biomedical advances that elucidated the etiology and pathogenesis of disease, psychosomatic medicine undertook a search for specific psychological causative factors. Dunbar was a pioneer here too, observing that patients with several medical disorders had distinctive personality traits which she concluded were etiological. Others developed similar formulations. The identified diseases were termed ‘psychosomatic.’ This effort peaked with the contributions of Franz Alexander (1950). As a psychoanalyst, he focused on unconscious processes, but his work was far more elaborate and sophisticated than others. Most importantly and enduringly, he clarified that no purely psychological mechanism could explain the physiological processes of internal organs. Instead, Alexander reported that ‘specific dynamic constellations’ of motivations, defenses, and emotions characterized patients with each of a series of ‘psychosomatic’ diseases, and that life experiences which matched the vulnerabilities inherent in the particular constellation precipitated that illness. He also linked each constellation to specific physiological processes mediated through autonomic neural pathways that plausibly explained the pathogenesis of that disease. The brilliance of this work captured the imagination of many, and it became a major force in psychosomatic medicine. Belief developed that psychotherapy might cure ‘psychosomatic diseases.’

2.4 The Last Third Of The Twentieth Century

More careful studies called into question much specificity research, which had been uncontrolled for observer bias, and utilized selected subjects. Skepticism grew and spread from specificity to involve all of psychosomatic medicine. Moreover, remarkable advances in biomedicine made other factors seem irrelevant: ‘With penicillin, who needs psychology?’ The influence of the field on the rest of medical science and practice withered.

Experiencing self-doubt and loss of external support, the field contracted and changed. Grand theories were eschewed. Research focused on better-defined questions and became methodologically tighter. Clinical studies became less common, especially those of therapy. Laboratory projects concentrated on elucidating basic psychophysiological relationships, often in normal subjects or even animals. If less exciting, this shift proved salutary, strengthening the scientific base and the quality of data.

Over time, the field recovered. Increasing studies of clinical problems embodied a new methodological rigor gained from models provided by the continuing basic research. A variety of subgroups within the psychosomatic society grew to a critical size and formed associations of their own, expanding work further. Acceptance by clinical biomedicine grew more slowly, however.

3. Theoretical Models Of Mind–Body Relationships

The evidence of correlations between psychological and somatic processes is extensive. But how can this be understood? Five explanatory models can be adduced from scientific reports.

3.1 Concomitant Effects

This model rejects causality in the relationship. It postulates that both the psychological and physical findings are products of another, precedent factor. Commonly, this is said to be genetic. Another explanation draws on the fact that life events often have multiple stimulus properties. Thus, one stimulus is said to cause the psychological findings while another produces the physical one.

3.2 Somato → Psychic Causation

This model posits that the relationship follows entirely from the effects of somatic processes on the mind. This is the traditional biomedical view, which sees all disease as ‘physical’ in nature and origin. In a modification, a feedback loop is added going back from the mind to the body, acknowledging that psychological changes may have physical effects, but only secondarily. (If this is true, why cannot they do so primarily?)

3.3 Psycho → Somatic Causation

This is the counterpart of the preceding. It states that psychological responses to external events cause the somatic changes. Most commonly, stress or strong emotions are invoked as intervening mechanisms. Weiner (1977) noted that it may be the Behavioral concomitants of the psychological processes which act on the body: ‘lifestyle factors’ such as smoking or substance abuse. Except for ‘specificity’ theories already described, the ‘choice’ of the resultant disease is usually left to other, biological factors. Again, secondary feedback is often added, bodily changes acting back on the mind.

3.4 Bidirectional Psycho → Somatic Causality

This is a combination of the last two models, allowing for causation in both directions and the feedback variations of each. Thus, longer causal chains are possible: … psycho … somato … psycho … somato … (etc.); but these remain linear.

3.5 The Modern Psychosomatic Model

This is a holistic model that includes all the preceding models and their variants, indicating that each explains some psychosomatic processes. Beyond this, it suggests that several are usually operating simultaneously, and to this adds that there are usually webs of relationships involving multiple stimulus–response linkages. To explain this, it invokes data about the operation of the mechanisms which integrate human function.

4. Integrative Mechanisms

From the biological side, the brain is the organ of the mind; mentation is subject to the same physical influences as all other organ functions. Beyond this, it is the body’s chief regulatory and coordinating organ. Inputs to the brain come from both the body and environment. Similarly, its output alters bodily functions and, via Behavior, the external world. These inputs from inside and out converge on many of the same brain centers, and the resultant messages alter both psychological and somatic functions.

From a psychological perspective, input consists not merely of sensations but of information organized into meanings. Meanings have individual as well as shared components. The same event or somatic message has different nuances, at least, for each person. The origins of these meanings, as well as those shared in the culture, derive from life experiences: they are learned. Some visceral responses to psychological stimuli also are ‘learned,’ but the learning that establishes these psychosomatic linkages is not the familiar, conscious process, but conditioning, of both Pavlovian and operant types. Most of this occurs in early life. Manipulations during early development can alter brain anatomy, neurotransmitters, and hormone levels, and modify subsequent stress responses.

When important meanings are attached to inputs, emotions are activated. These further affect body functions. The original response becomes a stimulus, too. In studying organismic function, what is defined as stimulus, and what as response, is arbitrary; it depends on what part of the process is being examined and at what point in time. Nor is this a simple chain of stimulus–response pairs. A network of actions follow, in which some responses affect one organ, some another, and some both, while some of these influences are stimulating and others inhibitory; and the succeeding responses become additional stimuli of the same variegated nature. Because many life events have more than one meaning, several such networks may be in process simultaneously. In most instances, then, a stimulus sets off a transacting web of responses and new stimuli with both psychological and somatic components.

Ordinarily, inhibitory processes finally dominate, and the person returns to a resting state. Sometimes, especially if the stimulus persists, inhibition may fail, so that the reaction continues or recurs. These ex- tended responses are homeostatically maladaptive, and likely to cause organ damage and disease.

The endocrine and immune systems also serve as integrators of organismic function, directly and in their inter-relationships with each other and the brain. Pituitary secretions, which control many other endocrines, are themselves subject to control by hormones and neurotransmitters from the hypothalamus. Psychological stress is associated with stimulation of adrenocortical hormones via the pituitary, as well as neurally mediated adrenaline release from the adrenal medulla. The distinction between neurotransmitters and hormones is blurred: active neuropeptides and neuroamines are present in many peripheral organs. Hormones act back on the brain to inhibit their own overproduction, but also modify cognitive and emotional states.

Stress also affects both cellular and humoral immune functions, partly through the action of adrenal steroids; depression may have especially potent effects. Animals subjected to a variety of psychological stressors have increased morbidity and mortality to injected pathogens along with changes in immune functions. Hypnosis can cure warts and alter the hypersensitivity reaction to injected antigens. The immune system can ‘learn’ too: immune responses to previously inert substances can be conditioned.

5. Basic Psychosomatic Concepts

5.1 Health And Disease

Psychosomatic medicine rests on the fundamental physiological concept of homeostasis: the human organism exists in a state of constantly dynamic equilibrium subject to both endogenous shifts and perturbations induced by an ever-changing environment. These trigger homeostatic responses which keep the changes from exceeding viable limits, thereby maintaining organismic integrity and permitting continuing function in the environment. If the change is excessive or the compensatory adjustments become so, damage results. When damage significantly impairs adaptation, disease occurs: disease is failure of adaptation. At any given moment, every person is more or less healthy and more or less diseased.

Psychosomatic medicine enhances this concept to take account of our inherent psychological and physiological properties as well as the sociocultural and informational content of the physical world. Moreover, although ‘disease’ may seem to have a fixed definition, different times, places, and persons have provided differing criteria for deciding what disease exists and whether one is sick at all. (Epilepsy in ancient Egyptian royalty was a manifestation of divinity.)

5.2 The Nature Of Disease; ‘Psychosomatic Disease’

Many physicians and others dichotomize disease states as ‘organic’ (physical) or ‘psychological’ (sometimes extending these, respectively, as ‘real’ or ‘imaginary’). Specificity theorists separated off ‘psychosomatic’ diseases in which psychogenesis played a uniquely central role. Psychosomatic medicine takes a wholly different view, conceptualizing every disease in every person as physical AND psychological. There are no ‘psychosomatic diseases’ because all diseases (and health) are psychosomatic.

Even in the simple instance of accidental injury, it is the overall impact of the event on the person, not just the injured part, that defines the illness—including instances where Behavioral factors played no causal role and no psychic ‘trauma’ results. The extent of pain, bleeding, swelling, etc., will be determined not only by the injury but by the victim’s entire prior psycho-physiological state; and the victim’s reactions to each of these effects, as well as his attitudes about the injured part and its functions, will alter that state. (For example, if anger arises, blood vessels may dilate, increasing bleeding.) The reactions of others, including health care providers, will also affect the psycho- physiological responses, as will the meanings of the treatment given. The somatic problems will be paramount medically, at least until late consequences require chronic care. Conceptually, however, even this extreme example of ‘pure physical illness’ can be fully understood only from a psychosomatic perspective. Optimal care will result only if health care providers are responsive to psychosocial needs throughout the course of treatment.

5.3 The Etiology Of Disease

Similarly, the causes of all diseases are psychosomatic. Take malaria for example. The plasmodium, usually mosquito-borne, is prerequisite. But, what is to be said of the US soldier in the tropics who contracts malaria after failing to take prescribed prophylactic medication or to sleep beneath issued netting, or who fishes in a swamp designated ‘off limits.’ It is no less valid to categorize such Behaviors as causative than the biological factors. Sociocultural factors, even political ones can enter in too. Examples include being a parentless child, or living in a culture ignorant of the mosquito’s role or under a political system too poor or uncaring to provide public health programs. Any such factors or other psychological events can be involved in any given person. Often several are.

The choice of an infectious disease was deliberate. Traditional medicine views these as the prototype of external causation. However, as Dubos (1960) indicated, all people are incubators for various microorganisms growing in their throat, intestine, etc.; and in these circumstances, why are people not sick all the time? The real question is why some people become sick with what they do, when they do. External factors explain only part of this. Host resistance variables, including differences in immunity, are necessarily involved too; and psychosocial factors affect these.

5.4 Pathogenesis; The Onset Of Disease

Among the psychosocial causes of illness, stress is the best documented. Selye (1950) demonstrated that the response to any noxious stimulus precipitated a nonspecific ‘General Adaptation Syndrome’ superimposed on the specific effects of the stimulus. He focused on the adrenocortical response, but it became clear that many other systems are involved in the syndrome, especially other hormones, the cardiovascular system, blood elements, including those active immunologically, and the psychological apparatus. Psychological stressors have proved as potent as physical ones. Indeed, some effects of physical stressors have proved to result from their psychological properties. Multiple clinical studies have documented that a variety of illnesses arise in the context of accumulated life stress.

Specific emotional states seem to operate in some circumstances. Anger and anxiety have been implicated, as well as depression, especially that associated with feelings of helplessness and hopelessness.

But disease is a process with a course that evolves variably, and often preceded by a period in which covert, even undetectable, lesions are developing; and a different superimposed process may be required for the disease to appear. Psychosocial factors have been implicated in each of these phases, but the specifics may differ from one to another, as Reiser (1975) clarified. For example, the gradual accretion of coronary atherosclerosis precedes thrombosis and infarction (‘heart attack’). Anger and depression have been implicated in infarction, whereas nonspecific stress has been related to alterations in cholesterol metabolism which can lead to atherosclerosis. Of course, the events that provoke emotions differ among people, depending on their psychological makeup. Research provides the broad possibilities, but only individual study can delineate the pathogenic process in a given person.

The specific ‘choice’ of illness likely rests in genetic and other purely biological factors in many instances. Individually patterned psychosomatic links may be implicated in others. Either may be responsible for ‘response specificity,’ the tendency for individuals to have particular somatic responses to a variety of stressors. Moreover, discredited but not disproved psychological ‘specificity’ remains a possibility. The remarkable consistency among various specificity theorists regarding the psychological characteristics in people with certain disorders remains suggestive. Formidable methodological problems must be over- come to study this adequately, but such research is merited.

6. Clinical Applications

The application of psychosomatic medicine to patient care rests primarily on its basic concepts. As every patient is partly unique, only by identifying the issues specific to that individual can appropriate care be provided. The fundamental need both in diagnosis and treatment is establishing a trusting professional relationship that allows patients to relax defenses and reveal personal information and feelings. Also required are interviewing skills to facilitate patients’ communications of their life experiences, along with physical symptoms and the life context of the latter. This must be accompanied by sufficient knowledge of psychology and social science to identify the significance of these communications, and also of major psychosomatic research findings in order to identify correlations likely to be significant.

Treatment requires the ability to utilize this understanding of the patient in order to select the biological therapies that are most suitable for the individual, and to provide this in a manner that helps that patient use it best. On the psychological side, the need is to quantify and facilitate the extent of emotional release most helpful and tolerable to that patient, as well as the level of insight to pursue the relevant psychosomatic correlations.

All this seems complex, and it is. But the knowledge and skills can be acquired in the course of existing professional education, provided that psychosomatic medicine is integrated throughout. This is true for all clinical specialties. As noted initially, there is no specialty of psychosomatic medicine.

At the height of interest in psychosomatic medicine, a cadre of experts began to serve as educators of physicians, medical students, and others in academic health centers. Most were psychiatrists, although some internists and other specialists participated. This became a new subspecialty, named ‘Consultation Liaison Psychiatry,’ as the service function became melded with education. In the late twentieth-century, however, as psychiatry became more biological and positioned within the biomedical model, the psychosomatic element diminished. Given the widespread public dissatisfaction with the ‘over(bio)scientific,’ ‘dehumanized’ quality of medical care, this has been a significant loss.

But clinical psychosomatic medicine should not be confused with humanism or doing ‘good’; it is applied science. Although its clinical practitioners act in a decent, humane fashion, it is not this but their skills and knowledge as applied scientists that make them effective.

7. Recent Developments

Substantial research has flourished under the rubrics of ‘health psychology,’ ‘Behavioral medicine,’ and ‘Behavioral psychology’ as well as psychosomatic medicine itself. Consequently, high-quality research has been advancing vigorously. Psychophysiological studies have begun to use highly sophisticated probe techniques to measure meaningful physiological parameters. A particularly new and useful approach involves the application of randomized clinical trials that elucidate both causal factors and the effectiveness of interventions in a number of diseases. New research designs have incorporated more real-life situations, particularly in research on stress. Information particularly relevant to actual clinical and life situations has emerged, giving meaningful promise for the future.

Bibliography:

  1. Alexander F 1950 Psychosomatic Medicine. Norton, New York
  2. Anderson N B, Armstead C A 1995 Toward understanding the association between socioeconomic status and health: A new challenge for the biopsychosocial approach. Psychosomatic Medicine 57: 213–25
  3. Cannon W B 1915 Bodily Changes in Pain, Hunger, Fear and Rage, 2nd edn. Appleton, New York
  4. Cannon W B 1932 The Wisdom of the Body. Norton, New York
  5. Cobb S 1976 Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine 38: 300–14
  6. Dubos R 1960 The Mirage of Health. Allen and Unwin, London
  7. Dunbar H F 1935 Emotions and Bodily Change. Columbia University Press, New York (see also the 4th edn., 1954)
  8. Engel G L 1962 Psychological Development in Health and Disease. Saunders, Philadelphia, PA
  9. Engel G L 1977 The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science 196: 129–36
  10. Grinker R R 1953 Psychosomatic Research. Norton, New York
  11. Kiecolt-Glaser J K, Glaser R 1988 Methodological issues in Behavioral immunology research with humans. Brain Behavior and Immunology 2: 67–78
  12. Kleinman A, Becker A E 1993 ‘Sociosomatics’: The contributions of anthropology to psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatic Medicine 60: 389–93
  13. Lazarus R S 1993 Coping theory and research: Past present and future. Psychosomatic Medicine 55: 234–47
  14. Mason J W 1968 Organization of psychoendocrine mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine 30(Suppl.): 365–808
  15. McEwen B S 1998 Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New England Journal of Medicine 338: 171–9
  16. Moynihan J A, Ader R 1996 Psychoneuroimmunology: Animal models of disease. Psychosomatic Medicine 58: 546–58
  17. Oken D 1989 Current theoretical concepts in psychosomatic medicine. In: Kaplan H I, Sadock B J (eds.) Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry V, 5th edn. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, MD, Chap. 25.2
  18. Reiser M F 1975 Changing theoretical concepts in psychosomatic medicine. In: Reiser M F (ed.) American Handbook of Psychiatry, Volume 4, 2nd edn. Basic Books, New York
  19. Reiser M F 1984 Mind, Brain, Body: Toward a Convergence of Psychoanalysis and Neurobiology. Basic Books, New York
  20. Selye H 1950 Stress. Acta, Montreal, Canada
  21. Stainbrook E 1952 Psychosomatic medicine in the 19th century. Psychosomatic Medicine 14: 211–27
  22. Weiner H 1977 Psychobiology and Human Disease. Elsevier, New York
  23. Weiner H, Hofer M A, Stunkard A J (eds.) 1981 Brain, Behavior and Bodily Disease. Raven Press, New York
Quality Of Life Research Paper
Psychoneuroimmunology Research Paper

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get 10% off with the 24START discount code!