Sample Cognitive Aging Research Paper. Browse other research paper examples and check the list of research paper topics for more inspiration. iResearchNet offers academic assignment help for students all over the world: writing from scratch, editing, proofreading, problem solving, from essays to dissertations, from humanities to STEM. We offer full confidentiality, safe payment, originality, and money-back guarantee. Secure your academic success with our risk-free services.
Cognitive aging is concerned with age-related changes in adulthood in the basic processes of learning and memory, as well as the complex higher order processes of language and intellectual competence or executive functioning. Although most of the literature has been concerned with explaining the mechanism of cognitive decline, there is also a substantial interest in issues such as compensation and the role of external support, including collaborative problem solving.
Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services
Get 10% OFF with 24START discount code
1. Definition Of Cognitive Aging
There have been two distinct traditions in the study of cognitive aging. The first grew out of experimental child psychology while the second derives from psychometric roots.
1.1 Experimental Study Of Memory Functions And Language
The concern in this literature is to explicate possible causal variables that would explain why many adults suffer memory loss and decline in the complex manipulation of language variables such as text processing. The typical approach here is to design experiments testing for the effects of single variables in carefully controlled laboratory settings requiring only limited numbers of subjects. Because there is often little interest in individual differences, or population parameters, study participants are typically drawn from convenience samples (McKay and Abrams 1996).
1.2 Descriptive Study Of Adult Intellectual Development
Descriptive studies of adult intellectual development often stem from the longitudinal follow up of samples first assessed in childhood or adolescence. Other such studies may represent carefully stratified samples from defined populations, first assessed at a particular life stage, whether in early adulthood or in early old age. Although descriptive studies often begin as cross-sectional inquiries, they are most frequently conducted as longitudinal analyses since the interest is often in individual differences in intraindividual change, or in the elucidation of typologies of individuals who follow different growth trajectories. These are frequently large-sample studies, and the use of correlational or quasi-experimental approaches is typical (Baltes et al. 1999, Schaie 1996b).
2. Methodological Issues
2.1 Age-Comparative vs. Age Change Designs
Much of the experimental cognitive aging literature is based on age-comparative studies, which typically contrast a group of young adults (typically college students) with convenience samples of community-dwelling older adults in their sixties and seventies. It should be recognized that such comparisons are fraught by the problem that it is often unreasonable to assume that the two age groups can be adequately matched for other status variables that might provide a rival explanation for any observed age difference on the dependent variable. This creates particular problems for identifying the mechanisms that may be implicated in age-related decline from young adult-hood into old age. Age-comparative designs are also inadequate in explaining individual differences in age changes. The latter can only be investigated by means of longitudinal paradigms (Schaie 1965).
2.2 The Role Of Response Speed
A number of theorists have argued that changes in the central nervous system are the primary common cause for the observed age-related declines in cognitive performance. In fact, there have been many published analyses that show a substantial reduction in age differences, if some measure or measures of reaction time or perceptual speed is partialled out of the relation between a given cognitive process and chronological age (Salthouse 1999). This issue is of particular concern because it is not clear whether the observed average increase in reaction time (generally assumed to be of the magnitude of approximately 1.6 from the early twenties to the late sixties), while reliably demonstrable in the laboratory, is of significance in many or most tasks of daily living.
3. Basic Findings From The Experimental Literature On Cognitive Aging
Most of this literature is cross-sectional in nature and usually consists of a comparison of convenience samples of young adults (often sophomore psychology students) and of community-dwelling older adults (often participants in adult education programs). The major findings regarding age differences in cognitive performance include the following.
3.1 Memory
It is currently thought that older persons are at a disadvantage in retrieving information from memory when the information to be retrieved is complex and when there are few cues or other environmental support. Hence, age differences are far greater in recall than in recognition of information. It is also thought that the magnitude of age difference in memory is far greater when a task involves effortful processing than when automatic processing is involved. Hence, greater age differences have been found for explicit than implicit memory. Older persons are also thought to have greater difficulty in integrating the context of information they are trying to remember. It is also thought that working memory capacity (i.e., the information kept in immediately accessible store) becomes reduced with increasing age. On the other hand there is little evidence for age differences in long-term storage. Memory deficits occurring with age include nonverbal tasks such as memory for spatial location, memory for faces, and for actions and activities. Studies of prospective memory (i.e., remembering something to be done in the future) suggest that older people do well in remembering simple and event-based tasks, but are at a disadvantage when tasks become complex or are time-based. In sum, it appears that age differences are known to increase in magnitude as a function of the processing requirement of a given task (Salthouse 1999, Smith 1996).
3.2 Language
Age-related differences in language behavior are closely related to the processes of encoding and retrieving verbal materials discussed above. But in addition there appear to be greater age differences in textual tasks that involve recent connections than in those that involve recollection of older connections. Language production also seems to be adversely affected in older persons under intense time pressure. The interesting tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon involving word-finding difficulty, however, seems to be more likely with infrequently used words. Significant age differences have also been found in language planning, that is, in planning what one intends to say and how to say it during language production. Hence, older persons are more likely to engage in hesitations, false starts, and repetitions. Age-linked deficits in story recall is thought to be more of a general deficit in connection formation than in specific communication ability. Older persons tend to benefit from textual material that provides priming of association because it contains learned semantically linked information (McKay and Abrams 1996).
4. Basic Findings From The Descriptive Literature On Age Changes In Intellectual Competence
Changes in intellectual competence over the adult life span have been studied primarily with either the Wechsler Intelligence Scale or with ability batteries derived from the Thurstonian Primary Mental Ability framework.
Distinctions are made between fluid abilities thought to be innate and crystallized abilities which involve the utilization of culturally acquired knowledge (Cattell 1963). More recently further distinctions have been introduced between the mechanics (or basic processes) of intellectual competence and the pragmatics that involve cultural mediation (Baltes et al. 1984).
What has been found in most longitudinal studies is that the adult life course of mental abilities is not uniform. The so-called fluid abilities (sometimes de- fined as cognitive mechanics or primitives) peak in early midlife and begin to decline in the early sixties. By contrast, crystallized abilities, or the pragmatics of intellectual competence that represent abilities acquired in a given cultural context (particularly verbal abilities), do not usually peak until the fifties are reached and begin to show significant decline only in the seventies and often show only minimal decline even in the eighties (Schaie 1996a). However, recent work in advanced old age suggests increasing convergence and steeper decline for both aspects of intellectual competence, probably caused by the in- creasing decline of sensory and central nervous system functions (Baltes and Lindenberger 1997, Baltes and Mayer 1999).
However, at any particular time, a cross-sectional snapshot may yield very different ability profiles because of the fact that subsequent population cohorts reach different asymptotes in midlife. For example, there has been a positive linear cohort trend in the twentieth century for inductive reasoning, the basic component of most problem-solving tasks, while there has been a negative trend in numeric skills. The magnitude of cohort differences in abilities since the 1950s has been comparable to the average age changes observed from young adulthood into the seventies. Hence, many older persons may appear to have declined markedly in comparison to young peers, even though the age difference may be primarily due to what might be called the obsolescence of earlier cohorts (Schaie 1996b).
Studies of individual differences suggest that while most persons have declined on some aspect of intellectual functioning from their own peak as the sixties are reached, that specific patterns of decline may well depend on complex patterns of individual life experience. Most healthy community-dwelling per-sons are able to maintain a high level of function until advanced old age (but see Baltes and Mayer 1999 for the consequences of sensory dysfunctions). Because most tasks of daily living represent complex combinations of basic cognitive processes, many individuals can maintain their abilities above the minimally necessary threshold level for independent functioning by often rather complex compensatory processes (cf. Baltes et al. 1984, Baltes et al. 1999).
5. Can Cognitive Aging Be Slowed Or Reversed?
Once the course of adult intellectual development had been described and a number of antecedents of individual differences had been identified, it then became useful for researchers to think about ways in which normal intellectual aging might be slowed or reversed.
In a number of laboratories (primarily in the USA and in Germany) cognitive training programs have been developed that have been applied in the lab-oratory, and more recently in cooperative multisite intervention trials. In contrast to training young children, where it can be assumed that new skills are conveyed, older adults are likely to have access to the skills being trained, but through disuse have lost their proficiency. Information from longitudinal studies is therefore particularly useful in distinguishing individuals who have declined from those who have remained stable. In the former, training is directed towards remediation of loss, while in the latter the enhancement of previous levels of functioning are sought with the intention of compensating for possibly cohort-based disadvantage of older persons.
Results from such cognitive interventions allow the conclusion that cognitive decline in old age, for many older persons, is likely to be a function of disuse rather than of the deterioration of the physiological or neural substrates of cognitive behavior. For example, a brief five-hour training program for persons over 65 resulted in average training gains of about one half SD on the abilities of spatial orientation and inductive reasoning. Of those for whom significant decrement could be documented over a 14-year period, roughly 40 percent were returned to the level at which they had functioned when first studied. The analyses of structural relationships among the ability measures prior to and after training further allow the conclusion that training does not result in qualitative changes in ability structures, and is thus highly specific to the targeted abilities. A seven-year follow up further demonstrated that those subjects who showed significant decline at initial training do remain at substantial advantage over untrained comparison groups (Willis and Schaie 1994). It should be noted, however, that while cognitive training may improve performance in the elderly and may function to reduce effects of age decrement, such training will also be effective in enhancing the performance of young adults such that age differences tend to remain robust (cf. Baltes and Kliegl 1992).
6. Other Related Topics In Cognitive Aging
Much of the work on cognitive aging in the past has been concerned with age-related development in the mechanics and basic processes of cognition. It should be recognized that current attention in the study of cognitive aging is turning to the discovery of how these basic processes operate within more complex domains. Of particular interest here are the study of wisdom (e.g., Baltes and Staudinger 1993, Sternberg 1990), the application of the basic processes to social cognition (e.g., Staudinger 1999), the development of expert systems (e.g. Charness and Bosman 1990), and in everyday problem solving (Willis 1996). The extensive literature on these topics is beyond the scope of this research paper.
Bibliography:
- Baltes P B, Dittmann-Kohli F, Dixon R A 1984 New perspectives on the development of intelligence in adulthood: Toward a dual process conception and a model of selective optimization with compensation. In: Baltes P B, Brim O G Jr (eds.) Lifespan Development and Behavior. Academic Press, New York, Vol. 6, pp. 33–76
- Baltes P B, Kliegl R 1992 Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psychology 28: 121–5
- Baltes P B, Lindenberger U 1997 Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window at the study of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging 12: 12–21
- Baltes P B, Mayer K U (eds.) 1999 The Berlin Aging Study: Aging from 70 to 100. Cambridge University Press, Cam-bridge, UK
- Baltes P B, Staudinger U M 1993 The search for a psychology of wisdom. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2: 75–80
- Baltes P B, Staudinger U M, Lindenberger U 1999 Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annual Review of Psychology 50: 471–507
- Cattell R B 1963 Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology 54: 1–22
- Charness N, Bosman E A 1990 Expertise and aging: Life in the lab. In: Hess T H (ed.) Aging and Cognition: Knowledge Organization and Utilization. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 343–85
- McKay D G, Abrams L 1996 Language, memory, and aging: Distributed deficits and the structure of new-versus-old connections. In: Birren J E, Schaie K W (eds.) Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 4th edn. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 251–65
- Salthouse T 1999 Theories of cognition. In: Bengtson V L, Schaie K W (eds.) Handbook of Theories of Aging. Springer, New York, pp. 196–208
- Schaie K W 1965 A general model for the study of developmental problems. Psychological Bulletin 64: 92–107
- Schaie K W 1996a Intellectual Development in Adulthood, The Seattle Longitudinal Study. Cambridge University Press, New York
- Schaie K W 1996b Intellectual functioning and aging. In: Birren J E, Schaie K W (eds.) Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 4th edn. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 266–86
- Schaie K W, Willis S L 1999 Theories of everyday competence. In: Bengtson V L, Schaie K W (eds.) Handbook of Theories of Aging. Springer, New York, pp. 174–95
- Smith A D 1996 Memory. In: Birren J E, Schaie K W (eds.) Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 4th edn. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 236–50
- Staudinger U M 1999 Social cognition and psychological approach to an art of life. In: Blanchard-Fields F, Hess T B (eds.) Social Cognition, Adult Development and aging. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 343–75
- Sternberg R J 1990 Wisdom and its relation to intelligence and creativity. In: Sternberg R J (ed.) Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development. Academic Press, New York, pp. 142–9
- Willis S L 1996 Everyday problem solving. In: Birren J E, Schaie K W (eds.) Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 4th edn. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 287–307
- Willis S L, Schaie K W 1994 Cognitive training in the normal elderly. In: Forette F, Christen Y, Boller F (eds.) Plasticite cerebrale et stimulation cognitive. Fondation Nationale de Gerontologie, Paris, pp. 91–113